KUBARK
Counterintelligence Interrogation
July 1963
- Introduction
- Explanation of Purpose
- Explanation of Organization
- Definitions
- Legal and Policy Considerations
- The Interrogator
- The Interrogatee
- Types of Sources: Intelligence Categories
- Types of Sources: Personality Categories
- Other Clues
- Screening and Other Preliminaries
- Screening
- Other Preliminary Procedures
- Summary
- Planning the Counterintelligence Interrogation
- The Nature of Counterintelligence Interrogation
- The Interrogation Plan
- The Specifics
- The Non-coercive Counterintelligence Interrogation
- General Remarks
- The Structure of the Interrogation
- The Opening
- The Reconnaissance
- The Detailed Questioning
- The Conclusion
- Techniques of Non-Coercive Interrogation of
Resistant Sources
- The Coercive Counterintelligence
Interrogation of Resistant Sources
- Restrictions
- The Theory of Coercion
- Arrest
- Detention
- Deprivation of Sensory Stimuli
- Threats and Fear
- Debility
- Pain
- Heightened Suggestibility and Hypnosis
- Narcosis
- The Detection of Malingering
- Conclusion
- Interrogator's Check List
- Descriptive Biliography
- Index
A. Explanation of Purpose
This manual cannot teach anyone how to be, or become, a good interrogator.
At best it can help readers to avoid the characteristic mistakes of
poor interrogators.
Its purpose is to provide guidelines for KUBARK interrogation, and
particularly the counterintelligence interrogation of resistant sources.
Designed as an aid for interrogators and others immediately concerned,
it is based largely upon the published results of extensive research,
including scientific inquiries conducted by specialists in closely related
subjects.
There is nothing mysterious about interrogation. It consists of no
more than obtaining needed information through responses to questions.
As is true of all craftsmen, some interrogators are more able than others;
and some of their superiority may be innate. But sound interrogation
nevertheless rests upon a knowledge of the subject matter and on certain
broad principles, chiefly psychological, which are not hard to understand.
The success of good interrogators depends in large measure upon their
use, conscious or not, of these principles and of processes and techniques
deriving from them. Knowledge of subject matter and of the basic principles
will not of itself create a successful interrogation, but it will make
possible the avoidance of mistakes that are characteristic of poor interrogation.
The purpose, then, is not to teach the reader how to be a good interrogator
but rather to tell him what he must learn in order to become a good
interrogator.
The interrogation of a resistant source who is a staff or agent member
of an Orbit intelligence or security service or of a clandestine Communist
organization is one of the most exacting of professional tasks. Usually
the odds still favor the interrogator, but they are sharply cut by the
training, experience, patience and toughness of the interrogatee. In
such circumstances the interrogator needs all the help that he can get.
And a principal source of aid today is scientific findings. The intelligence
service which is able to bring pertinent, modern knowledge to bear upon
its problems enjoys huge advantages over a service which conducts its
clandestine business in eighteenth century fashion. It is true that
American psychologists have devoted somewhat more attention to Communist
interrogation techniques, particularly "brainwashing", than to U. S.
practices. Yet they have conducted scientific inquiries into many subjects
that are closely related to interrogation: the effects of debility and
isolation, the polygraph, reactions to pain and fear, hypnosis and heightened
suggestibility, narcosis, etc. This work is of sufficient importance
and relevance that it is no longer possible to discuss interrogation
significantly without reference to the psychological research conducted
in the past decade. For this reason a major purpose of this study is
to focus relevant scientific findings upon CI interrogation. Every effort
has been made to report and interpret these findings in our own language,
in place of the terminology employed by the psychologists.
This study is by no means confined to a resume and interpretation
of psychological findings. The approach of the psychologists is customarily
manipulative; that is, they suggest methods of imposing controls or
alterations upon the interrogatee from the outside. Except within the
Communist frame of reference, they have paid less attention to the creation
of internal controls -- i.e., conversion of the source, so that voluntary
cooperation results. Moral considerations aside, the imposition of external
techniques of manipulating people carries with it the grave risk of
later lawsuits, adverse publicity, or other attempts to strike back.
Contents
B. Explanation of Organization
This study moves from the general topic of interrogation per se (Parts
I, II, III, IV,
V, and VI) to planning the counterintelligence
interrogation (Part VII) to the CI interrogation
of resistant sources (Parts VIII, IX,
and X). The definitions, legal considerations, and
discussions of interrogators and sources, as well as Section VI on screening
and other preliminaries, are relevant to all kinds of interrogations.
Once it is established that the source is probably a counterintelligence
target (in other words, is probably a member of a foreign intelligence
or security service, a Communist, or a part of any other group engaged
in clandestine activity directed against the national security), the
interrogation is planned and conducted accordingly. The CI interrogation
techniques are discussed in an order of increasing intensity as the
focus on source resistance grows sharper. The last section, on do's
and dont's, is a return to the broader view of the opening parts; as
a check-list, it is placed last solely for convenience.
Contents
Most of the intelligence terminology employed here which may once have
been ambiguous has been clarified through usage or through KUBARK instructions.
For this reason definitions have been omitted for such terms as burn
notice, defector, escapee, and refugee. Other definitions have been
included despite a common agreement about meaning if the significance
is shaded by the context.
1. Assessment: the analysis and synthesis of information, usually
about a person or persons, for the purpose of appraisal. The assessment
of individuals is based upon the compilation and use of psychological
as well as biographic detail.
2. Bona fides: evidence or reliable information about identity,
personal (including intelligence) history, and intentions or good faith.
3. Control: the capacity to generate, alter, or halt human
behavior by implying, citing, or using physical or psychological means
to ensure compliance with direction. The compliance may be voluntary
or involuntary. Control of an interrogatee can rarely be established
without control of his environment.
4. Counterintelligence interrogation: an interrogation (see
#7) designed to obtain information about hostile clandestine activities
and persons or groups engaged therein. KUBARK CI interrogations are
designed, almost invariably, to yield information about foreign intelligence
and security services or Communist organizations. Because security is
an element of counterintelligence, interrogations conducted to obtain
admissions of clandestine plans or activities directed against KUBARK
or PBPRIME security are also CI interrogations. But unlike a police
interrogation, the CI interrogation is not aimed at causing the interrogatee
to incriminate himself as a means of bringing him to trial. Admissions
of complicity are not, to a CI service, ends in themselves but merely
preludes to the acquisition of more information.
5. Debriefing: obtaining information by questioning a controlled
and witting source who is normally a willing one.
6. Eliciting: obtaining information, without revealing intent
or exceptional interest, through a verbal or written exchange with a
person who may be willing or unwilling to provide what is sought and
who may or may not be controlled.
7. Interrogation: obtaining information by direct questioning
of a person or persons under conditions which are either partly or fully
controlled by the questioner or are believed by those questioned to
be subject to his control. Because interviewing, debriefing, and eliciting
are simpler methods of obtaining information from cooperative subjects,
interrogation is usually reserved for sources who are suspect, resistant,
or both.
8. Intelligence interview: obtaining information, not customarily
under controlled conditions, by questioning a person who is aware of
the nature and perhaps of the significance of his answers but who is
ordinarily unaware of the purposes and specific intelligence affiliations
of the interviewer.
Contents
The legislation which founded KUBARK specifically denied it any law-enforcement
or police powers. Yet detention in a controlled environment and perhaps
for a lengthy period is frequently essential to a successful counterintelligence
interrogation of a recalcitrant source. [approx. three lines deleted]
This necessity, obviously, should be determined as early as possible.
The legality of detaining and questioning a person, and of the methods
employed, [approx. 10 lines deleted]
Detention poses the most common of the legal problems. KUBARK has
no independent legal authority to detain anyone against his will, [approx.
4 lines deleted] The haste in which some KUBARK interrogations have
been conducted has not always been the product of impatience. Some security
services, especially those of the Sino-Soviet Bloc, may work at leisure,
depending upon time as well as their own methods to melt recalcitrance.
KUBARK usually cannot. Accordingly, unless it is considered that the
prospective interrogatee is cooperative and will remain so indefinitely,
the first step in planning an interrogation is to determine how long
the source can be held. The choice of methods depends in part upon the
answer to this question.
[approx. 15 lines deleted]
The handling and questioning of defectors are subject to the provisions
of [one or two words deleted] Directive No. 4: to its related Chief/KUBARK
Directives, principally [approx. 1/2 line deleted] Book Dispatch [one
or two words deleted] and to pertinent [one or two words deleted]. Those
concerned with the interrogation of defectors, escapees, refugees, or
repatriates should know these references.
The kinds of counterintelligence information to be sought in a CI
interrogation are stated generally in Chief/KUBARK Directive and in
greater detail in Book Dispatch [approx. 1/3 line deleted].
The interrogation of PBPRIME citizens poses special problems. First,
such interrogations should not be conducted for reasons lying outside
the sphere of KUBARK' s responsibilities. For example, the [approx.
2/3 line deleted] but should not normally become directly involved.
Clandestine activity conducted abroad on behalf of a foreign power by
a private PBPRIME citizens does fall within KUBARK's investigative and
interrogative responsibilities. However, any investigation, interrogation,
or interview of a PBPRIME citizen which is conducted abroad because
it be known or suspected that he is engaged in clandestine activities
directed against PBPRIME security interests requires the prior and personal
approval of Chief/KUDESK or of his deputy.
Since 4 October 1961, extraterritorial application has been given
to the Espionage Act, making it henceforth possible to prosecute in
the Federal Courts any PBPRIME citizen who violates the statutes of
this Act in foreign countries. ODENVY has requested that it be informed,
in advance if time permits, if any investigative steps are undertaken
in these cases. Since KUBARK employees cannot be witnesses in court,
each investigation must be conducted in such a manner that evidence
obtained may be properly introduced if the case comes to trial. [approx.
1 line deleted] states policy and procedures for the conduct of investigations
of PBPRIME citizens abroad.
Interrogations conducted under compulsion or duress are especially
likely to involve illegality and to entail damaging consequences for
KUBARK. Therefore prior Headquarters approval at the KUDOVE level must
be obtained for the interrogation of any source against his will and
under any of the following circumstances:
1. If bodily harm is to be inflicted.
2. If medical, chemical, or electrical methods or materials are to
be used to induce acquiescence.
3. [approx. 3 lines deleted]
The CI interrogator dealing with an uncooperative interrogatee who
has been well-briefed by a hostile service on the legal restrictions
under which ODYOKE services operate must expect some effective delaying
tactics. The interrogatee has been told that KUBARK will not hold him
long, that he need only resist for a while. Nikolay KHOKHLOV, for example,
reported that before he left for Frankfurt am Main on his assassination
mission, the following thoughts coursed through his head: "If I should
get into the hands of Western authorities, I can become reticent, silent,
and deny my voluntary visit to Okolovich. I know I will not be tortured
and that under the procedures of western law I can conduct myself boldly."
(17) [The footnote numerals in this text are keyed to the numbered bibliography
at the end.] The interrogator who encounters expert resistance should
not grow flurried and press; if he does, he is likelier to commit illegal
acts which the source can later use against him. Remembering that time
is on his side, the interrogator should arrange to get as much of it
as he needs.
Contents
A number of studies of interrogation discuss qualities said to be desirable
in an interrogator. The list seems almost endless - a professional manner,
forcefulness, understanding and sympathy, breadth of general knowledge,
area knowledge, "a practical knowledge of psychology", skill in the
tricks of the trade, alertness, perseverance, integrity, discretion,
patience, a high I.Q., extensive experience, flexibility, etc., etc.
Some texts even discuss the interrogator's manners and grooming, and
one prescribed the traits considered desirable in his secretary.
A repetition of this catalogue would serve no purpose here, especially
because almost all of the characteristics mentioned are also desirable
in case officers, agents, policemen, salesmen, lumberjacks, and everybody
else. The search of the pertinent scientific literature disclosed no
reports of studies based on common denominator traits of successful
interrogators or any other controlled inquiries that would invest these
lists with any objective validity.
Perhaps the four qualifications of chief importance to the interrogator
are (1) enough operational training and experience to permit quack recognition
of leads; (2) real familiarity with the language to be used; (3) extensive
background knowledge about the interrogatee's native country (and intelligence
service, if employed by one); and (4) a genuine understanding of the
source as a person.
[approx. 1/2 line deleted] stations, and even a few bases can call
upon one or several interrogators to supply these prerequisites, individually
or as a team. Whenever a number of interrogators is available, the percentage
of successes is increased by careful matching of questioners and sources
and by ensuring that rigid prescheduling does not prevent such matching.
Of the four traits listed, a genuine insight into the source's character
and motives is perhaps most important but least common. Later portions
of this manual explore this topic in more detail. One general observation
is introduced now, however, because it is considered basic to the establishment
of rapport, upon which the success of non-coercive interrogation depends.
The interrogator should remember that he and the interrogatee are
often working at cross-purposes not because the interrogates is malevolently
withholding or misleading but simply because what he wants front the
situation is not what the interrogator wants. The interrogator's goal
is to obtain useful information -- facts about which the interrogatee
presumably have acquired information. But at the outset of the interrogation,
and perhaps for a long time afterwards, the person being questioned
is not greatly concerned with communicating his body of specialized
information to his questioner; he is concerned with putting his best
foot forward. The question uppermost in his mind, at the beginning,
is not likely to be "How can I help PBPRIME?" but rather "What sort
of impression am I making?" and, almost immediately thereafter, "What
is going to happen to me now?" (An exception is the penetration agent
or provocateur sent to a KUBARK field installation after training in
withstanding interrogation. Such an agent may feel confident enough
not to be gravely concerned about himself. His primary interest, from
the beginning, may be the acquisition of information about the interrogator
and his service.)
The skilled interrogator can save a great deal of time by understanding
the emotional needs of the interrogates. Most people confronted by an
official -- and dimly powerful -- representative of a foreign power
will get down to cases much faster if made to feel, from the start,
that they are being treated as individuals. So simple a matter as greeting
an interrogatee by his name at the opening of the session establishes
in his mind the comforting awareness that he is considered as a person,
not a squeezable sponge. This is not to say that egotistic types should
be allowed to bask at length in the warmth of individual recognition.
But it is important to assuage the fear of denigration which afflicts
many people when first interrogated by making it clear that the individuality
of the interrogatee is recognized. With this common understanding established,
the interrogation can move on to impersonal matters and will not later
be thwarted or interrupted -- or at least not as often -- by irrelevant
answers designed not to provide facts but to prove that the interrogatee
is a respectable member of the human race.
Although it is often necessary to trick people into telling what we
need to know, especially in CI interrogations, the initial question
which the interrogator asks of himself should be, "How can I make him
want to tell me what he knows?" rather than "How can I trap him into
disclosing what he knows?" If the person being questioned is genuinely
hostile for ideological reasons, techniques of manipulation are in order.
But the assumption of hostility -- or at least the use of pressure tactics
at the first encounter -- may make difficult subjects even out of those
who would respond to recognition of individuality and an initial assumption
of good will.
Another preliminary comment about the interrogator is that normally
he should not personalize. That is, he should not be pleased, flattered,
frustrated, goaded, or otherwise emotionally and personally affected
by the interrogation. A calculated display of feeling employed for a
specific purpose is an exception; but even under these circumstances
the interrogator is in full control. The interrogation situation is
intensely inter-personal; it is therefore all the more necessary to
strike a counter-balance by an attitude which the subject clearly recognizes
as essentially fair and objective. The kind of person who cannot help
personalizing, who becomes emotionally involved in the interrogation
situation, may have chance (and even spectacular) successes as an interrogator
but is almost certain to have a poor batting average.
It is frequently said that the interrogator should be "a good judge
of human nature." In fact, [approx. 3 lines deleted] (3) This study
states later (page "Great attention has been given to the degree to
which persons are able to make judgements from casual observations regarding
the personality characteristics of another. The consensus of research
is that with respect to many kinds of judgments, at least some judges
perform reliably better than chance...." Nevertheless, "... the level
of reliability in judgments is so low that research encounters difficulties
when it seeks to determine who makes better judgments...." (3) In brief,
the interrogator is likelier to overestimate his ability to judge others
than to underestimate it, especially if he has had little or no training
in modern psychology. It follows that errors in assessment and in handling
are likelier to result from snap judgments based upon the assumption
of innate skill in judging others than from holding such judgments in
abeyance until enough facts are known.
There has been a good deal of discussion of interrogation experts
vs. subject-matter experts. Such facts as are available suggest that
the latter have a slight advantage. But for counterintelligence purposes
the debate is academic. [approx. 5 lines deleted]
It is sound practice to assign inexperienced interrogators to guard
duty or to other supplementary tasks directly related to interrogation,
so that they can view the process closely before taking charge. The
use of beginning interrogators as screeners (see part
VI) is also recommended.
Although there is some limited validity in the view, frequently expressed
in interrogation primers, that the interrogation is essentially a battle
of wits, the CI interrogator who encounters a skilled and resistant
interrogatee should remember that a wide variety of aids can be made
available in the field or from Headquarters. (These are discussed in
Part VIII.) The intensely personal nature of the
interrogation situation makes it all the more necessary that the KUBARK
questioner should aim not for a personal triumph but for his true goal
-- the acquisition of all needed information by any authorized means.
___________________
*The interrogator should be supported whenever possible by qualified
analysts' review of his daily "take"; experience has shown that such
a review will raise questions to be put and points to be clarified and
lead to a thorough coverage of the subject in hand.
Contents
A. Types Of Sources: Intelligence Categories
From the viewpoint of the intelligence service the categories of persons
who most frequently provide useful information in response to questioning
are travellers; repatriates; defectors, escapees, and refugees; transferred
sources; agents, including provocateurs, double agents, and penetration
agents; and swindlers and fabricators.
1. Travellers are usually interviewed, debriefed, or queried
through eliciting techniques. If they are interrogated, the reason is
that they are known or believed to fall into one of the following categories.
2. Repatriates are sometimes interrogated, although other techniques
are used more often. The proprietary interests of the host government
will frequently dictate interrogation by a liaison service rather than
by KUBARK. If KUBARK interrogates, the following preliminary steps are
taken:
a. A records check, including local and Headquarters traces.
b . Testing of bona fides .
c. Determination of repatriate's kind and level of access while outside
his own country.
d. Preliminary assessment of motivation (including political orientation),
reliability, and capability as observer and reporter.
e. Determination of all intelligence or Communist
relationships, whether with a service or party of the repatriate's
own country, country of detention, or another. Full particulars are
needed.
3. Defectors, escapees, and refugees are normally interrogated at
sufficient length to permit at least a preliminary testing of bona fides
. The experience of the post-war years has demonstrated that Soviet
defectors (1) almost never defect solely or primarily because of inducement
by a Western service, (2) usually leave the USSR for personal rather
than ideological reasons, and (3) are often RIS agents.
[approx. 9 lines deleted]
All analyses of the defector-refugee flow have shown that the Orbit
services are well-aware of the advantages offered by this channel as
a means of planting their agents in target countries.
[approx. 14 lines deleted]
4. Transferred sources referred to KUBARK by another service
for interrogation are usually sufficiently well-known to the transferring
service so that a file has been opened. Whenever possible, KUBARK should
secure a copy of the file or its full informational equivalent before
accepting custody.
5. Agents are more frequently debriefed than interrogated.
[approx. 3 lines deleted] as an analytic tool. If it is then established
or strongly suspected that the agent belongs to one of the following
categories, further investigation and, eventually, interrogation usually
follow.
a. Provocateur. Many provocation agents are walk-ins posing
as escapees, refugees, or defectors in order to penetrate emigre groups,
ODYOKE intelligence, or other targets assigned by hostile services.
Although denunciations by genuine refugees and other evidence of information
obtained from documents, local officials, and like sources may result
in exposure, the detection of provocation frequently depends upon skilled
interrogation. A later section of this manual deals with the preliminary
testing of bona fides . But the results of preliminary testing are often
inconclusive, and detailed interrogation is frequently essential to
confession and full revelation. Thereafter the provocateur may be questioned
for operational and positive intelligence as well as counterintelligence
provided that proper cognizance is taken of his status during the questioning
and later, when reports are prepared.
b. Double agent. The interrogation of DA's frequently follows
a determination or strong suspicion that the double is "giving the edge"
to the adversary service. As is also true for the interrogation of provocateurs,
thorough preliminary investigation will pay handsome dividends when
questioning gets under way. In fact, it is a basic principle of interrogation
that the questioner should have at his disposal, before querying starts,
as much pertinent information as can be gathered without the knowledge
of the prospective interrogatee.
[2/3 of page deleted]
d. Swindlers and fabricators are usually interrogated for prophylactic
reasons, not for counterintelligence information. The purpose is the
prevention or nullification of damage to KUBARK, to other ODYOKE services
Swindlers and fabricators have little of CI significance to communicate
but are notoriously skillful timewasters. Interrogation of them is usually
inconclusive and, if prolonged, unrewarding. The professional peddler
with several IS contacts may prove an exception; but he will usually
give the edge to a host security service because otherwise he cannot
function with impunity.
Contents
B. Types of Sources: Personality Categories
The number of systems devised for categorizing human beings is large,
and most of them are of dubious validity. Various categorical schemes
are outlined in treatises on interrogation. The two typologies most
frequently advocated are psychologic-emotional and geographic-cultural.
Those who urge the former argue that the basic emotional-psychological
patterns do not vary significantly with time, place, or culture. The
latter school maintains the existence of a national character and sub-national
categories, and interrogation guides based on this principle recommend
approaches tailored to geographical cultures.
It is plainly true that the interrogation source cannot be understood
in a vacuum, isolated from social context. It is equally true that some
of the most glaring blunders in interrogation (and other operational
processes ) have resulted from ignoring the source's background. Moreover,
emotional-psychological schematizations sometimes present atypical extremes
rather than the kinds of people commonly encountered by interrogators.
Such typologies also cause disagreement even among professional psychiatrists
and psychologists. Interrogators who adopt them and who note in an interrogatee
one or two of the characteristics of "Type A" may mistakenly assign
the source to Category A and assume the remaining traits.
On the other hand, there are valid objections to the adoption of cultural-geographic
categories for interrogation purposes (however valid they may be as
KUCAGE concepts). The pitfalls of ignorance of the distinctive culture
of the source have "[approx. 4 lines deleted]
[approx. 8 lines deleted]." (3)
The ideal solution would be to avoid all categorizing. Basically,
all schemes for labelling people are wrong per se; applied arbitrarily,
they always produce distortions. Every interrogator knows that a real
understanding of the individual is worth far more than a thorough knowledge
of this or that pigeon-hole to which he has been consigned. And for
interrogation purposes the ways in which he differs from the abstract
type may be more significant than the ways in which he conforms.
But KUBARK does not dispose of the time or personnel to probe the
depths of each source's individuality. In the opening phases of interrogation,
or in a quick interrogation, we are compelled to make some use of the
shorthand of categorizing, despite distortions. Like other interrogation
aides, a scheme of categories is useful only if recognized for what
it is -- a set of labels that facilitate communication but are not the
same as the persons thus labelled. If an interrogatee lies persistently,
an interrogator may report and dismiss him as a "pathological liar."
Yet such persons may possess counterintelligence (or other) information
quite equal in value to that held by other sources, and the interrogator
likeliest to get at it is the man who is not content with labelling
but is as interested in why the subject lies as in what he lies about.
With all of these reservations, then, and with the further observation
that those who find these psychological-emotional categories pragmatically
valuable should use them and those who do not should let them alone,
the following nine types are described. The categories are based upon
the fact that a person's past is always reflected, however dimily, in
his present ethics and behavior. Old dogs can learn new tricks but not
new ways of learning them. People do change, but what appears to be
new behavior or a new psychological pattern is usually just a variant
on the old theme.
It is not claimed that the classification system presented here is
complete; some interrogatees will not fit into any one of the groupings.
And like all other typologies, the system is plagued by overlap, so
that some interrogatees will show characteristics of more than one group.
Above all, the interrogator must remember that finding some of the characteristics
of the group in a single source does not warrant an immediate conclusion
that the source "belongs to" the group, and that even correct labelling
is not the equivalent of understanding people but merely an aid to understanding.
The nine major groups within the psychological-emotional category
adopted for this handbook are the following.
1. The orderly-obstinate character. People in this category
are characteristically frugal, orderly, and cold; frequently they are
quite intellectual. They are not impulsive in behavior. They tend to
think things through logically and to act deliberately. They often reach
decisions very slowly. They are far less likely to make real personal
sacrifices for a cause than to use them as a temporary means of obtaining
a permanent personal gain. They are secretive and disinclined to confide
in anyone else their plans and plots, which frequently concern the overthrow
of some form of authority. They are also stubborn, although they may
pretend cooperation or even believe that they are cooperating. They
nurse grudges.
The orderly-obstinate character considers himself superior to other
people. Sometimes his sense of superiority is interwoven with a kind
of magical thinking that includes all sorts of superstitions and fantasies
about controlling his environment. He may even have a system of morality
that is all his own. He sometimes gratifies his feeling of secret superiority
by provoking unjust treatment. He also tries, characteristically, to
keep open a line of escape by avoiding any real commitment to anything.
He is -- and always has been -- intensely concerned about his personal
possessions. He is usually a tightwad who saves everything, has a strong
sense of propriety, and is punctual and tidy. His money and other possessions
have for him a personalized quality; they are parts of himself. He often
carries around shiny coins, keepsakes, a bunch of keys, and other objects
having for himself an actual or symbolic value.
Usually the orderly-obstinate character has a history of active rebellion
in childhood, of persistently doing the exact opposite of what he is
told to do. As an adult he may have learned to cloak his resistance
and become passive-aggressive, but his determination to get his own
way is unaltered. He has merely learned how to proceed indirectly if
necessary. The profound fear and hatred of authority, persisting since
childhood, is often well-concealed in adulthood, For example, such a
person may confess easily and quickly under interrogation, even to acts
that he did not commit, in order to throw the interrogator off the trail
of a significant discovery (or, more rarely, because of feelings of
guilt).
The interrogator who is dealing with an orderly-obstinate character
should avoid the role of hostile authority. Threats and threatening
gestures, table-pounding, pouncing on evasions or lies, and any similarly
authoritative tactics will only awaken in such a subject his old anxieties
and habitual defense mechanisms. To attain rapport, the interrogator
should be friendly. It will probably prove rewarding if the room and
the interrogator look exceptionally neat. Orderly-obstinate interrogatees
often collect coins or other objects as a hobby; time spent in sharing
their interests may thaw some of the ice. Establishing rapport is extremely
important when dealing with this type. [approx 3 lines deleted] (3)
2. The optimistic character. This kind of source is almost
constantly happy-go-lucky, impulsive, inconsistent, and undependable.
He seems to enjoy a continuing state of well-being. He may be generous
to a fault, giving to others as he wants to be given to. He may become
an alcoholic or drug addict. He is not able to withstand very much pressure;
he reacts to a challenge not by increasing his efforts but rather by
running away to avoid conflict. His convictions that "something will
turn up", that "everything will work out all right", is based on his
need to avoid his own responsibility for events and depend upon a kindly
fate.
Such a person has usually had a great deal of over-indulgence in early
life. He is sometimes the youngest member of a large family, the child
of a middle-aged woman (a so-called "change-of-life baby"). If he has
met severe frustrations in later childhood, he may be petulant, vengeful,
and constantly demanding.
As interrogation sources, optimistic characters respond best to a
kindly, parental approach. If withholding, they can often be handled
effectively by the Mutt-and-Jeff technique discussed later in this paper.
Pressure tactics or hostility will make them retreat inside themselves,
whereas reassurance will bring them out. They tend to seek promises,
to cast the interrogator in the role of protector and problem-solver;
and it is important that the interrogator avoid making any specific
promises that cannot be fulfilled, because the optimist turned vengeful
is likely to prove troublesome.
3. The greedy, demanding character. This kind of person affixes
himself to others like a leech and clings obsessively. Although extremely
dependent and passive, he constantly demands that others take care of
him and gratify his wishes. If he considers himself wronged, he does
not seek redress through his own efforts but tries to persuade another
to take up the cudgels in his behalf -- "let's you and him fight." His
loyalties are likely to shift whenever he feels that the sponsor whom
he has chosen has let him down. Defectors of this type feel aggrieved
because their desires were not satisfied in their countries of origin,
but they soon feel equally deprived in a second land and turn against
its government or representatives in the same way. The greedy and demanding
character is subject to rather frequent depressions. He may direct a
desire for revenge inward, upon himself; in extreme cases suicide may
result.
The greedy, demanding character often suffered from very early deprivation
of affection or security. As an adult he continues to seek substitute
parents who will care for him as his own, he feels, did not.
The interrogator dealing with a greedy, demanding character must be
careful not to rebuff him; otherwise rapport will be destroyed. On the
other hand, the interrogator must not accede to demands which cannot
or should not be met. Adopting the tone of an understanding father or
big brother is likely to make the subject responsive. If he makes exorbitant
requests, an unimportant favor may provide a satisfactory substitute
because the demand arises not from a specific need but as an expression
of the subject's need for security. He is likely to find reassuring
any manifestation of concern for his well-being.
In dealing with this type -- and to a considerable extent in dealing
with any of the types herein listed -- the interrogator must be aware
of the limits and pitfalls of rational persuasion. If he seeks to induce
cooperation by an appeal to logic, he should first determine whether
the source's resistance is based on logic. The appeal will glance off
ineffectually if the resistance is totally or chiefly emotional rather
than rational. Emotional resistance can be dissipated only by emotional
manipulation.
4. The anxious, self-centered character. Although this person
is fearful, he is engaged in a constant struggle to conceal his fears.
He is frequently a daredevil who compensates for his anxiety by pretending
that there is no such thing as danger. He may be a stunt flier or circus
performer who "proves" himself before crowds. He may also be a Don Juan.
He tends to brag and often lies through hunger for approval or praise.
As a soldier or officer he may have been decorated for bravery; but
if so, his comrades may suspect that his exploits resulted from a pleasure
in exposing himself to danger and the anticipated delights of rewards,
approval, and applause. The anxious, self-centered character is usually
intensely vain and equally sensitive.
People who show these characteristics are actually unusually fearful.
The causes of intense concealed anxiety are too complex and subtle to
permit discussion of the subject in this paper.
Of greater importance to the interrogator than the causes is the opportunity
provided by concealed anxiety for successful manipulation of the source.
His desire to impress will usually be quickly evident. He is likely
to be voluble. Ignoring or ridiculing his bragging, or cutting him short
with a demand that he get down to cases, is likely to make him resentful
and to stop the flow. Playing upon his vanity, especially by praising
his courage, will usually be a successful tactic if employed skillfully.
Anxious, self-centered interrogatees who are withholding significant
facts, such as contact with a hostile service, are likelier to divulge
if made to feel that the truth will not be used to harm them and if
the interrogator also stresses the callousness and stupidity of the
adversary in sending so valiant a person upon so ill-prepared a mission.
There is little to be gained and much to be lost by exposing the nonrelevant
lies of this kind of source. Gross lies about deeds of daring, sexual
prowess, or other "proofs" of courage and manliness are best met with
silence or with friendly but noncommittal replies unless they consume
an inordinate amount of time. If operational use is contemplated, recruitment
may sometimes be effected through such queries as, "I wonder if you
would be willing to undertake a dangerous mission."
5. The guilt-ridden character. This kind of person has a strong
cruel, unrealistic conscience. His whole life seems devoted to reliving
his feelings of guilt. Sometimes he seems determined to atone; at other
times he insists that whatever went wrong is the fault of somebody else.
In either event he seeks constantly some proof or external indication
that the guilt of others is greater than his own. He is often caught
up completely in efforts to prove that he has been treated unjustly.
In fact, he may provoke unjust treatment in order to assuage his conscience
through punishment. Compulsive gamblers who find no real pleasure in
winning but do find relief in losing belong to this class. So do persons
who falsely confess to crimes. Sometimes such people actually commit
crimes in order to confess and be punished. Masochists also belong in
this category.
The causes of most guilt complexes are real or fancied wrongs done
to parents or others whom the subject felt he ought to love and honor.
As children such people may have been frequently scolded or punished.
Or they may have been "model" children who repressed all natural hostilities.
The guilt-ridden character is hard to interrogate. He may "confess"
to hostile clandestine activity, or other acts of interest to KUBARK,
in which he was not involved. Accusations levelled at him by the interrogator
are likely to trigger such false confessions. Or he may remain silent
when accused, enjoying the "punishment." He is a poor subject for LCFLUTTER.
The complexities of dealing with conscience-ridden interrogatees vary
so widely from case to case that it is almost impossible to list sound
general principles. Perhaps the best advice is that the interrogator,
once alerted by information from the screening process (see Part
VI) or by the subject's excessive preoccupation with moral judgements,
should treat as suspect and subjective any information provided by the
interrogatee about any matter that is of moral concern to him. Persons
with intense guilt feelings may cease resistance and cooperate if punished
in some way, because of the gratification induced by punishment.
6. The character wrecked by success is closely related to the
guilt-ridden character. This sort of person cannot tolerate success
and goes through life failing at critical points. He is often accident-prone.
Typically he has a long history of being promising and of almost completing
a significant assignment or achievement but not bringing it off. The
character who cannot stand success enjoys his ambitions as long as they
remain fantasies but somehow ensures that they will not be fulfilled
in reality. Acquaintances often feel that his success is just around
the corner, but something always intervenes. In actuality this something
is a sense of guilt, of the kind described above. The person who avoids
success has a conscience which forbids the pleasures of accomplishment
and recognition. He frequently projects his guilt feelings and feels
that all of his failures were someone else's fault. He may have a strong
need to suffer and may seek danger or injury.
As interrogatees these people who "cannot stand prosperity" pose no
special problem unless the interrogation impinges upon their feelings
of guilt or the reasons for their past failures. Then subjective distortions,
not facts, will result. The successful interrogator will isolate this
area of unreliability.
7. The schizoid or strange character lives in a world of fantasy
much of the time. Sometimes he seems unable to distinguish reality from
the realm of his own creating. The real world seems to him empty and
meaningless, in contrast with the mysteriously significant world that
he has made. He is extremely intolerant of any frustration that occurs
in the outer world and deals with it by withdrawal into the interior
realm.
He has no real attachments to others, although he may attach symbolic
and private meanings or values to other people.
Children reared in homes lacking in ordinary affection and attention
or in orphanages or state-run communes may become adults who belong
to this category. Rebuffed in early efforts to attach themselves to
another, they become distrustful of attachments and turn inward. Any
link to a group or country will be undependable and, as a rule, transitory.
At the same time the schizoid character needs external approval. Though
he retreats from reality, he does not want to feel abandoned.
As an interrogatee the schizoid character is likely to lie readily
to win approval. He will tell the interrogator what he thinks the interrogator
wants to hear in order to win the award of seeing a smile on the interrogator's
face. Because he is not always capable of distinguishing between fact
and fantasy, he may be unaware of lying. The desire for approval provides
the interrogator with a handle. Whereas accusations of lying or other
indications of disesteem will provoke withdrawal from the situation,
teasing the truth out of the schizoid subject may not prove difficult
if he is convinced that he will not incur favor through misstatements
or disfavor through telling the truth.
Like the guilt-ridden character, the schizoid character may be an
unreliable subject for testing by LCFLUTTER because his internal needs
lead him to confuse fact with fancy. He is also likely to make an unreliable
agent because of his incapacity to deal with facts and to form real
relationships.
8. The exception believes that the world owes him a great deal.
He feels that he suffered a gross injustice, usually early in life,
and should be repaid. Sometimes the injustice was meted out impersonally,
by fate, as a physical deformity, an extremely painful illness or operation
in childhood, or the early loss of one parent or both. Feeling that
these misfortunes were undeserved, the exceptions regard them as injustices
that someone or something must rectify. Therefore they claim as their
right privileges not permitted others. When the claim is ignored or
denied, the exceptions become rebellious, as adolescents often do. They
are convinced that the justice of the claim is plain for all to see
and that any refusal to grant it is willfully malignant.
When interrogated, the exceptions are likely to make demands for money,
resettlement aid, and other favors -- demands that are completely out
of proportion to the value of their contributions. Any ambiguous replies
to such demands will be interpreted as acquiescence. Of all the types
considered here, the exception is likeliest to carry an alleged injustice
dealt him by KUBARK to the newspapers or the courts.
The best general line to follow in handling those who believe that
they are exceptions is to listen attentively (within reasonable timelimits)
to their grievances and to make no commitments that cannot be discharged
fully. Defectors from hostile intelligence services, doubles, provocateurs,
and others who have had more than passing contact with a Sino-Soviet
service may, if they belong to this category, prove unusually responsive
to suggestions from the interrogator that they have been treated unfairly
by the other service. Any planned operational use of such persons should
take into account the fact that they have no sense of loyalty to a common
cause and are likely to turn aggrievedly against superiors.
9. The average or normal character is not a person wholly lacking
in the characteristics of the other types. He may, in fact, exhibit
most or all of them from time to time. But no one of them is persistently
dominant; the average man's qualities of obstinacy, unrealistic optimism,
anxiety, and the rest are not overriding or imperious except for relatively
short intervals. Moreover, his reactions to the world around him are
more dependent upon events in that world and less the product of rigid,
subjective patterns than is true of the other types discussed.
Contents
C. Other Clues
[approx. 4 lines deleted]
The true defector (as distinguished from the hostile agent in defector's
guise) is likely to have a history of opposition to authority. The sad
fact is that defectors who left their homelands because they could not
get along with their immediate or ultimate superiors are also likely
to rebel against authorities in the new environment (a fact which usually
plays an important part in redefection). Therefore defectors are likely
to be found in the ranks of the orderly-obstinate, the greedy and deriding,
the schizoids, and the exceptions.
Experiments and statistical analyses performed at the University of
Minnesota concerned the relationships among anxiety and affiliative
tendencies (desire to be with other people), on the one hand, and the
ordinal position (rank in birth sequence) on the other. Some of the
findings, though necessarily tentative and speculative, have some relevance
to interrogation. (30). As is noted in the bibliography, the investigators
concluded that isolation typically creates anxiety, that anxiety intensifies
the desire to be with others who share the same fear, and that only
and first-born children are more anxious and less willing or able to
withstand pain than later-born children. Other applicable hypotheses
are that fear increases the affiliative needs of first-born and only
children much more than those of the later-born. These differences are
more pronounced in persons from small families then in those who grew
up in large families. Finally, only children are much likelier to hold
themselves together and persist in anxiety-producing situations than
are the first-born, who more frequently try to retreat. In the other
major respects - intensity of anxiety and emotional need to affiliate
- no significant differences between "firsts" and "onlies" were discovered.
It follows that determining the subject's "ordinal position" before
questioning begins may be useful to the interrogator. But two cautions
are in order. The first is that the findings are, at this stage, only
tentative hypotheses. The second is that even if they prove accurate
for large groups, the data are like those in actuarial tables; they
have no specific predictive value for individuals.
Contents
VI. Screening and Other Preliminaries
A. Screening
[approx. 2/3 line deleted] some large stations are able to conduct
preliminary psychological screening before interrogation starts. The
purpose of screening is to provide the interrogator, in advance, with
a reading on the type and characteristics of the interrogatee. It is
recommended that screening be conducted whenever personnel and facilities
permit, unless it is reasonably certain that the interrogation will
be of minor importance or that the interrogatee is fully cooperative.
Screening should be conducted by interviewers, not interrogators;
or at least the subjects should not be screened by the same KUBARK personnel
who will interrogate them later.
[approx. 10 lines deleted]
Other psychological testing aids are best administered by a trained
psychologist. Tests conducted on American POW's returned to U. S. jurisdiction
in Korea during the Big and Little Switch suggest that prospective interrogatees
who show normal emotional responsiveness on the Rorschach and related
tests are likelier to prove cooperative under interrogation than are
those whose responses indicate that they are apathetic and emotionally
withdrawn or barren. Extreme resisters, however, share the response
characteristics of collaborators; they differ in the nature and intensity
of motivation rather than emotions. "An analysis of objective test records
and biographical information is a sample of 759 Big Switch repatriates
revealed that men who had collaborated differed from men who had not
in the following ways: the collaborators were older, had completed more
years of school, scored higher on intelligence tests administered after
repatriation, had served longer in the Army prior to capture, and scored
higher on the Psychopathic Deviate Scale - pd.... However, the 5 percent
of the noncollaborator sample who resisted actively - who were either
decorated by the Army or considered to be 'reactionaries' by the Chinese
- differed from the remaining group in precisely the same direction
as the collaborator group and could not be distinguished from this group
on any variable except age; the resisters were older than the collaborators."
(33)
Even a rough preliminary estimate, if valid, can be a boon to the
interrogator because it will permit him to start with generally sound
tactics from the beginning - tactics adapted to the personality of the
source. Dr. Moloney has expressed the opinion, which we may use as an
example of this, that the AVH was able to get what it wanted from Cardinal
Mindszenty because the Hungarian service adapted its interrogation methods
to his personality. "There can be no doubt that Mindszenty's preoccupation
with the concept of becoming secure and powerful through the surrender
of self to the greatest power of them all - his God idea - predisposed
him to the response elicited in his experience with the communist intelligence.
For him the surrender of self-system to authoritarian-system was natural,
as was the very principle of martyrdom." (28)
The task of screening is made easier by the fact that the screener
is interested in the subject, not in the information which he may possess.
Most people -- even many provocation agents who have been trained to
recite a legend -- will speak with some freedom about childhood events
and familial relationships. And even the provocateur who substitutes
a fictitious person for his real father will disclose some of his feelings
about his father in the course of detailing his story about the imaginary
substitute. If the screener has learned to put the potential source
at ease, to feel his way along in each case, the source is unlikely
to consider that a casual conversation about himself if dangerous .
The screener is interested in getting the subject to talk about himself.
Once the flow starts, the screener should try not to stop it by questions,
gestures, or other interruptions until sufficient information has been
revealed to permit a rough determination of type. The subject is likeliest
to talk freely if the screener's manner is friendly and patient. His
facial expression should not reveal special interest in any one statement;
he should just seem sympathetic and understanding. Within a short time
most people who have begun talking about themselves go back to early
experiences, so that merely by listening and occasionally making a quiet,
encouraging remark the screener can learn a great deal. Routine questions
about school teachers, employers, and group leaders, for example, will
lead the subject to reveal a good deal of how he feels about his parents,
superiors, and others of emotional consequence to him because of associative
links in his mind.
It is very helpful if the screener can imaginatively place himself
in the subject's position. The more the screener knows about the subject's
native area and cultural background, the less likely is he to disturb
the subject by an incongruous remark. Such comments as, "That must have
been a bad time for you and your family," or "Yes, I can see why you
were angry," or "It sounds exciting" are sufficiently innocuous not
to distract the subject, yet provide adequate evidence of sympathetic
interest. Tasking the subject's side against his enemies serves the
same purpose, and such comments as "That was unfair; they had no right
to treat you that way" will aid rapport and stimulate further revelations.
It is important that gross abnormalities be spotted during the screening
process. Persons suffering from severe mental illness will show major
distortions, delusions, or hallucinations and will usually give bizarre
explanations for their behavior. Dismissal or prompt referral of the
mentally ill to professional specialists will save time and money.
The second and related purpose of screening is to permit an educated
guess about the source's probable attitude toward the interrogation.
An estimate of whether the interrogatee will be cooperative or recalcitrant
is essential to planning because very different methods are used in
dealing with these two types.
At stations or bases which cannot conduct screening in the formal
sense, it is still worth-while to preface any important interrogation
with an interview of the source, conducted by someone other than the
interrogator and designed to provide a maximum of evaluative information
before interrogation commences.
Unless a shock effect is desired, the transition from the screening
interview to the interrogation situation should not be abrupt. At the
first meeting with the interrogatee it is usually a good idea for the
interrogator to spend some time in the same kind of quiet, friendly
exchange that characterized the screening interview. Even though the
interrogator now has the screening product, the rough classification
by type, he needs to understand the subject in his own terms. If he
is immediately aggressive, he imposes upon the first interrogation session
(and to a diminishing extent upon succeeding sessions) too arbitrary
a pattern. As one expert has said, "Anyone who proceeds without consideration
for the disjunctive power of anxiety in human relationships will never
learn interviewing." (34)
Contents
B. Other Preliminary Procedures
[approx. 2 lines deleted] The preliminary handling of other types
of interrogation sources is usually less difficult. It suffices for
the present purpose to list the following principles:
1. All available pertinent information ought to be assembled and studied
before the interrogation itself is planned, much less conducted. An
ounce of investigation may be worth a pound of questions.
2. A distinction should be drawn as soon as possible between sources
who will be sent to [approx. 1/2 line deleted site organized and equipped
for interrogation and those whose interrogation will be completed
by the base or station with which contact is first established.
3. The suggested procedure for arriving at a preliminary assessment
of walk-ins remains the same [approx. 4 lines deleted]
The key points are repeated here for ease of reference. These preliminary
tests are designed to supplement the technical examination of a walk-in's
documents, substantive questions about claimed homeland or occupation,
and other standard inquiries. The following questions, if asked, should
be posed as soon as possible after the initial contact, while the walk-in
is still under stress and before he has adjusted to a routine.
a. The walk-in may be asked to identify all relatives and friends
in the area, or even the country, in which PBPRIME asylum is first requested.
Traces should be run speedily. Provocation agents are sometimes directed
to "defect" in their target areas, and friends or relatives already
in place may be hostile assets.
b. At the first interview the questioner should be on the alert for
phrases or concepts characteristic of intelligence or CP activity and
should record such leads whether it is planned to follow them by interrogation
on the spot [approx. 1 line deleted]
c. LCFLUTTER should be used if feasible. If not, the walk-in may be
asked to undergo such testing at a later date. Refusals should be recorded,
as well as indications that the walk-in has been briefed on the technique
by another service. The manner as well as the nature of the walk-in's
reaction to the proposal should be noted.
d. If LCFLUTTER, screening. investigation, or any other methods do
establish a prior intelligence history, the following minimal information
should be obtained:
[approx. 1/3 page deleted] (7
[approx. 1/2 page deleted]
h. [approx. 3 lines deleted]
[entire page redacted, except for "4." about 3/4 of the way down the
page]
[approx. 4 lines deleted]
5. All documents that have a bearing on the planned interrogation
merit study. Documents from Bloc countries, or those which are in any
respect unusual or unfamiliar, are customarily sent to the proper field
or headquarters component for technical analysis.
6. If during screening or any other pre-interrogation phase it is
ascertained that the source has been interrogated before, this fact
should be made known to the interrogator. Agents, for example, are accustomed
to being questioned repeatedly and professionally. So are persons who
have been arrested several times. People who have had practical training
in being interrogated become sophisticated subjects, able to spot uncertainty,
obvious tricks, and other weaknesses.
Contents
C. Summary
Screening and the other preliminary procedures will help the interrogator
- and his base, station, [one or two words deleted] to decide whether
the prospective source (1) is likely to possess useful counterintelligence
because of association with a foreign service or Communist Party and
(2) is likely to cooperate voluntarily or not. Armed with these estimates
and with whatever insights screening has provided into the personality
of the source, the interrogator is ready to plan.
Contents
VII. Planning the Counterintelligence Interrogation
A. The Nature of Counterintelligence Interrogation
The long-range purpose of CI interrogation is to get from the source
all the useful counterintelligence information that he has. The short-range
purpose is to enlist his cooperation toward this end or, if he is resistant,
to destroy his capacity for resistance and replace it with a cooperative
attitude. The techniques used in nullifying resistance, inducing compliance,
and eventually eliciting voluntary cooperation are discussed in Part
VIII of this handbook.
No two interrogations are the same. Every interrogation is shaped
definitively by the personality of the source - and of the interrogator,
because interrogation is an intensely interpersonal process. The whole
purpose of screening and a major purpose of the first stage of the interrogation
is to probe the strengths and weaknesses of the subject. Only when these
have been established and understood does it become possible to plan
realistically.
Planning the CI interrogation of a resistant source requires an understanding
(whether formalized or not) of the dynamics of confession. Here Horowitz's
study of the nature of confession is pertinent. He starts by asking
why confessions occur at all. "Why not always brazen it out when confronted
by accusation? Why does a person convict himself through a confession,
when, at the very worst, no confession would leave him at least as well
off (and possibly better off)...?" He answers that confessions obtained
without duress are usually the product of the following conditions:
1. The person is accused explicitly or implicitly and feels accused.
2. As a result his psychological freedom - the extent to which he
feels able to do what he wants to - is curtailed. This feeling need
not correspond to confinement or any other external reality.
3. The accused feels defensive because he is on unsure ground. He
does not know how much the accuser knows. As a result the accused "has
no formula for proper behavior, no role if you will, that he can utilize
in this situation."
4. He perceives the accuser as representing authority. Unless he believes
that the accuser's powers far exceed his own, he is unlikely to feel
hemmed in and defensive. And if he "perceives that the accusation is
backed by 'real' evidence, the ratio of external forces to his own forces
is increased and the person's psychological position is now more precarious.
It is interesting to note that in such situations the accused tends
toward over response, or exaggerated response; to hostility and emotional
display; to self-righteousness, to counter accusation, to defense....
"
5. He must believe that he is cut off from friendly or supporting
forces. If he does, he himself becomes the only source of his "salvation."
6. "Another condition, which is most probably necessary, though not
sufficient for confession, is that the accused person feels guilt. A
possible reason is that a sense of guilt promotes self-hostility." It
should be equally clear that if the person does not feel guilt he is
not in his own mind guilty and will not confess to an act which others
may regard as evil or wrong and he, in fact, considers correct. Confession
in such a case can come only with duress even where all other conditions
previously mentioned may prevail."
7. The accused, finally, is pushed far enough along the path toward
confession that it is easier for him to keep going than to turn back.
He perceives confession as the only way out of his predicament and into
freedom. (15)
Horowitz has been quoted and summarized at some length because it
is considered that the foregoing is a basically sound account of the
processes that evoke confessions from sources whose resistance is not
strong at the outset, who have not previously-been confronted with detention
and interrogation, and who have not been trained by an adversary intelligence
or security service in resistance techniques. A fledgling or disaffected
Communist or agent, for example, might be brought to confession and
cooperation without the use of any external coercive forces other than
the interrogation situation itself, through the above-described progression
of subjective events.
It is important to understand that interrogation, as both situation
and process, does of itself exert significant external pressure upon
the interrogatee as long as he is not permitted to accustom himself
to it. Some psychologists trace this effect back to infantile relationships.
Meerlo, for example, says that every verbal relationship repeats to
some degree the pattern of early verbal relationships between child
and parent. (27) An interrogatee, in particular, is likely to see the
interrogator as a parent or parent-symbol, an object of suspicion and
resistance or of submissive acceptance. If the interrogator is unaware
of this unconcsious process, the result can be a confused battle of
submerged attitudes, in which the spoken words are often merely a cover
for the unrelated struggle being waged at lower levels of both personalities.
On the other hand, the interrogator who does understand these facts
and who knows how to turn them to his advantage may not need to resort
to any pressures greater than those that flow directly from the interrogation
setting and function.
Obviously, many resistant subjects of counterintelligence interrogation
cannot be brought to cooperation, or even to compliance, merely through
pressures which they generate within themselves or through the unreinforced
effect of the interrogation situation. Manipulative techniques - still
keyed to the individual but brought to bear upon him from outside himself
- then become necessary. It is a fundamental hypothesis of this handbook
that these techniques, which can succeed even with highly resistant
sources, are in essence methods of inducing regression of the personality
to whatever earlier and weaker level is required for the dissolution
of resistance and the inculcation of dependence. All of the techniques
employed to break through an interrogation roadblock, the entire spectrum
from simple isolation to hypnosis and narcosis, are essentially ways
of speeding up the process of regression. As the interrogatee slips
back from maturity toward a more infantile state, his learned or structured
personality traits fall away in a reversed chronological order, so that
the characteristics most recently acquired - which are also the characteristics
drawn upon by the interrogatee in his own defense - are the first to
go. As Gill and Brenman have pointed out, regression is basically a
loss of autonomy. (13)
Another key to the successful interrogation of the resisting source
is the provision of an acceptable rationalization for yielding. As regression
proceeds, almost all resisters feel the growing internal stress that
results from wanting simultaneously to conceal and to divulge. To escape
the mounting tension, the source may grasp at any face-saving reason
for compliance - any explanation which will placate both his own conscience
and the possible wrath of former superiors and associates if he is returned
to Communist control. It is the business of the interrogator to provide
the right rationalization at the right time. Here too the importance
of understanding the interrogatee is evident; the right rationalization
must be an excuse or reason that is tailored to the source's personality.
The interrogation process is a continuum, and everything that takes
place in the continuum influences all subsequent events. The continuing
process, being interpersonal, is not reversible. Therefore it is wrong
to open a counterintelligence interrogation experimentally, intending
to abandon unfruitful approaches one by one until a sound method is
discovered by chance. The failures of the interrogator, his painful
retreats from blind alleys, bolster the confidence of the source and
increase his ability to resist. While the interrogator is struggling
to learn from the subject the facts that should have been established
before interrogation started, the subject is learning more and more
about the interrogator.
Contents
B. The Interrogation Plan
Planning for interrogation is more important than the specifics of
the plan. Because no two interrogations are alike, the interrogation
cannot realistically be planned from A to Z, in all its particulars,
at the outset. But it can and must be planned from A to F or A to M.
The chances of failure in an unplanned CI interrogation are unacceptably
high. Even worse, a "dash-on-regardless" approach can ruin the prospects
of success even if sound methods are used later.
The intelligence category to which the subject belongs, though not
determinant for planning purposes, is still of some significance. The
plan for the interrogation of a traveller differs from that for other
types because the time available for questioning is often brief. The
examination of his bona fides , accordingly, is often less searching.
He is usually regarded as reasonably reliable if his identity and freedom
from other intelligence associations have been established, if records
checks do not produce derogatory information, if his account of his
background is free of omissions or discrepancies suggesting significant
withholding, if he does not attempt to elicit information about the
questioner or his sponsor, and if he willingly provides detailed information
which appears reliable or is established as such.
[approx. 2 lines deleted]
[approx. 5 lines deleted]
Defectors can usually be interrogated unilaterally, at least for a
time. Pressure for participation will usually come [approx. 1/2 line
deleted] from an ODYOKE intelligence component. The time available for
unilateral testing and exploitation should be calculated at the outset,
with a fair regard for the rights and interests of other members of
the intelligence community. The most significant single fact to be kept
in mind when planning the interrogation of Soviet defectors is that
a certain percentage of them have proven to be controlled agents; estimates
of this percentage have ranged as high as [one or two words deleted]
during a period of several years after 1955. (22)
KUBARK's lack of executive powers is especially significant if the
interrogation of a suspect agent or of any other subject who is expected
to resist is under consideration. As a general rule, it is difficult
to succeed in the CI interrogation of a resistant source unless the
interrogating service can control the subject and his environment for
as long as proves necessary.
[approx. 20 lines deleted]
[1/3 of page deleted]
Contents
C. The Specifics
1. The Specific Purpose
Before questioning starts, the interrogator has clearly in mind what
he wants to learn, why he thinks the source has the information, how
important it is, and how it can best be obtained. Any confusion here,
or any questioning based on the premise that the purpose will take shape
after the interrogation is under way, is almost certain to lead to aimlessness
and final failure. If the specific goals cannot be discerned clearly,
further investigation is needed before querying starts.
2. Resistance
The kind and intensity of anticipated resistance is estimated. It
is useful to recognize in advance whether the information desired would
be threatening or damaging in any way to the interests of the interrogates.
If so, the interrogator should consider whether the same information,
or confirmation of it, can be gained from another source. Questioning
suspects immediately, on a flimsy factual basis, will usually cause
waste of time, not save it. On the other hand, if the needed information
is not sensitive from the subject's viewpoint, merely asking for it
is usually preferable to trying to trick him into admissions and thus
creating an unnecessary battle of wits.
The preliminary psychological analysis of the subject makes it easier
to decide whether he is likely to resist and, if so, whether his resistance
will be the product of fear that his personal interests will be damaged
or the result of the non-cooperative nature of orderly-obstinate and
related types. The choice of methods to be used in overcoming resistance
is also determined by the characteristics of the interrogatee.
3. The Interrogation Setting
The room in which the interrogation is to be conducted should be free
of distractions. The colors of walls, ceiling, rugs, and furniture should
not be startling. Pictures should be missing or dull. Whether the furniture
should include a desk depends not upon the interrogator's convenience
but rather upon the subject's anticipated reaction to connotations of
superiority and officialdom. A plain table may be preferable. An overstuffed
chair for the use of the interrogatee is sometimes preferable to a straight-backed,
wooden chair because if he is made to stand for a lengthy period or
is otherwise deprived of physical comfort, the contrast is intensified
and increased disorientation results. Some treatises on interrogation
are emphatic about the value of arranging the lighting so that its source
is behind the interrogator and glares directly at the subject. Here,
too, a flat rule is unrealistic. The effect upon a cooperative source
is inhibitory, and the effect upon a withholding source may be to make
him more stubborn. Like all other details, this one depends upon the
personality of the interrogatee.
Good planning will prevent interruptions. If the room is also used
for purposes other than interrogation, a "Do Not Disturb" sign or its
equivalent should hang on the door when questioning is under way. The
effect of someone wandering in because he forgot his pen or wants to
invite the interrogator to lunch can be devastating. For the same reason
there should not be a telephone in the room; it is certain to ring at
precisely the wrong moment. Moreover, it is a visible link to the outside;
its presence makes a subject feel less cut-off, better able to resist.
The interrogation room affords ideal conditions for photographing
the interrogatee without his knowledge by concealing a camera behind
a picture or elsewhere.
If a new safehouse is to be used as the interrogation site, it should
be studied carefully to be sure that the total environment can be manipulated
as desired. For example, the electric current should be known in advance,
so that transformers or other modifying devices will be on hand if needed.
Arrangements are usually made to record the interrogation, transmit
it to another room, or do both. Most experienced interrogators do not
like to take notes. Not being saddled with this chore leaves them free
to concentrate on what sources say, how they say it, and what else they
do while talking or listening. Another reason for avoiding note-taking
is that it distracts and sometimes worries the interrogatee. In the
course of several sessions conducted without note-taking, the subject
is likely to fall into the comfortable illusion that he is not talking
for the record. Another advantage of the tape is that it can be played
back later. Upon some subjects the shock of hearing their own voices
unexpectedly is unnerving. The record also prevents later twistings
or denials of admissions. [approx. 6 lines deleted] A recording is also
a valuable training aid for interrogators, who by this means can study
their mistakes and their most effective techniques. Exceptionally instructuve
interrogations, or selected portions thereof, can also be used in the
training of others.
If possible, audio equipment should also be used to transmit the proceedings
to another room, used as a listening post. The main advantage of transmission
is that it enables the person in charge of the interrogation to note
crucial points and map further strategy, replacing one interrogator
with another, timing a dramatic interruption correctly, etc. It is also
helpful to install a small blinker bulb behind the subject or to arrange
some other method of signalling the interrogator, without the source's
knowledge, that the questioner should leave the room for consultation
or that someone else is about to enter.
4. The Participants
Interrogatees are normally questioned separately. Separation permits
the use of a number of techniques that would not be possible otherwise.
It also intensifies in the source the feeling of being cut off from
friendly aid. Confrontation of two or more suspects with each other
in order to produce recriminations or admissions is especially dangerous
if not preceded by separate interrogation sessions which have evoked
compliance from one of the interrogatees, or at least significant admissions
involving both. Techniques for the separate interrogations of linked
sources are discussed in Part IX.
The number of interrogators used for a single interrogation case varies
from one man to a large team. The size of the team depends on several
considerations, chiefly the importance of the case and the intensity
of source resistance. Although most sessions consist of one interrogator
and one interrogatee, some of the techniques described later call for
the presence of two, three, or four interrogators. The two-man team,
in particular, is subject to unintended antipathies and conflicts not
called for by assigned roles. Planning and subsequent conduct should
eliminate such cross-currents before they develop, especially because
the source will seek to turn them to his advantage.
Team members who are not otherwise engaged can be employed to best
advantage at the listening post. Inexperienced interrogators find that
listening to the interrogation while it is in progress can be highly
educational.
Once questioning starts, the interrogator is called upon to function
at two levels. He is trying to do two seemingly contradictory things
at once: achieve rapport with the subject but remain an essentially
detached observer. Or he may project himself to the resistant interrogatee
as powerful and ominous (in order to eradicate resistance and create
the necessary conditions for rapport) while remaining wholly uncommitted
at the deeper level, noting the significance of the subjects reactions
and the effectiveness of his own performance. Poor interrogators often
confuse this bi-level functioning with role-playing, but there is a
vital difference. The interrogator who merely pretends, in his surface
performance, to feel a given emotion or to hold a given attitude toward
the source is likely to be unconvincing; the source quickly senses the
deception. Even children are very quick to feel this kind of pretense.
To be persuasive, the sympathy or anger must be genuine; but to be useful,
it must not interfere with the deeper level of precise, unaffected observation.
Bi-level functioning is not difficult or even unusual; most people act
at times as both performer and observer unless their emotions are so
deeply involved in the situation that the critical faculty disintegrates.
Through experience the interrogator becomes adept in this dualism. The
interrogator who finds that he has become emotionally involved and is
no longer capable of unimpaired objectivity should report the facts
so that a substitution can be made. Despite all planning efforts to
select an interrogator whose age, background, skills, personality, and
experience make him the best choice for the job, it sometimes happens
that both questioner and subject feel, when they first meet, an immediate
attraction or antipathy which is so strong that a change of interrogators
quickly becomes essential. No interrogator should be reluctant to notify
his superior when emotional involvement becomes evident. Not the reaction
but a failure to report it would be evidence of a lack of professionalism.
Other reasons for changing interrogators should be anticipated and
avoided at the outset. During the first part of the interrogation the
developing relationship between the questioner and the initially uncooperative
source is more important than the information obtained; when this relationship
is destroyed by a change of interrogators, the replacement must start
nearly from scratch. In fact, he starts with a handicap, because exposure
to interrogation will have made the source a more effective resister.
Therefore the base, station, [one or two words deleted] should not assign
as chief interrogator a person whose availability will end before the
estimated completion of the case.
5. The Timing
Before interrogation starts, the amount of time probably required
and probably available to both interrogator and interrogatee should
be calculated. If the subject is not to be under detention, his normal
schedule is ascertained in advance, so that he will not have to be released
at a critical point because he has an appointment or has to go to work.
Because pulling information from a recalcitrant subject is the hard
way of doing business, interrogation should not begin until all pertinent
facts available from overt and from cooperative sources have been assembled.
Interrogation sessions with a resistant source who is under detention
should not be held on an unvarying schedule. The capacity for resistance
is diminished by disorientation. The subject may be left alone for days;
and he may be returned to his cell, allowed to sleep for five minutes,
and brought back to an interrogation which is conducted as though eight
hours had intervened. The principle is that sessions should be so planned
as to disrupt the source's sense of chronological order.
6. The Termination
The end of an interrogation should be planned before questioning starts.
The kinds of questions asked, the methods employed, and even the goals
sought may be shaped by what will happen when the end is reached. [approx.
3 lines deleted] If he is to be released upon the local economy, perhaps
blacklisted as a suspected hostile agent but not subjected to subsequent
counterintelligence surveillance, it is important to avoid an inconclusive
ending that has warned the interrogates of our doubts but has established
nothing. The poorest interrogations are those that trail off into an
inconclusive nothingness.
A number of practical terminal details should also be considered in
advance. Are the source's documents to be returned to him, and will
they be available in time? Is he to be paid? If he is a fabricator or
hostile agent, has he been photographed and fingerprinted? Are subsequent
contacts necessary or desirable, and have recontact provisions been
arranged? Has a quit-claim been obtained?
As was noted at the beginning of this section, the successful interrogation
of a strongly resistant source ordinarily involves two key processes:
the calculated regression of the interrogatee and the provision of an
acceptable rationalization. If these two steps have been taken, it becomes
very important to clinch the new tractability by means of conversion.
In other words, a subject who has finally divulged the information sought
and who has been given a reason for divulging which salves his self-esteem,
his conscience, or both will often be in a mood to take the final step
of accepting the interrogator' s values and making common cause with
him. If operational use is now contemplated, conversion is imperative.
But even if the source has no further value after his fund of information
has been mined, spending some extra time with him in order to replace
his new sense of emptiness with new values can be good insurance. All
non-Communist services are bothered at times by disgruntled exinterrogatees
who press demands and threaten or take hostile action if the demands
are not satisfied. Defectors in particular, because they are often hostile
toward any kind of authority, cause trouble by threatening or bringing
suits in local courts, arranging publication of vengeful stories, or
going to the local police. The former interrogatee is especially likely
to be a future trouble-maker if during interrogation he was subjected
to a form of compulsion imposed from outside himself. Time spent, after
the interrogation ends, in fortifying the source's sense of acceptance
in the interrogator's world may be only a fraction of the time required
to bottle up his attempts to gain revenge. Moreover, conversion may
create a useful and enduring asset. (See also remarks in VIII
B 4.)
Contents
VIII. The Non-Coercive Counterintelligence Interrogation
A. General Remarks
The term non-coercive is used above to denote methods of interrogation
that are not based upon the coercion of an unwilling subject through
the employment of superior force originating outside himself. However,
the non-coercive interrogation is not conducted without pressure. On
the contrary, the goal is to generate maximum pressure, or at least
as much as is needed to induce compliance. The difference is that the
pressure is generated inside the interrogatee. His resistance is sapped,
his urge to yield is fortified, until in the end he defeats himself.
Manipulating the subject psychologically until he becomes compliant,
without applying external methods of forcing him to submit, sounds harder
than it is. The initial advantage lies with the interrogator. From the
outset, he knows a great deal more about the source than the source
knows about him. And he can create and amplify an effect of omniscience
in a number of ways. For example, he can show the interrogatee a thick
file bearing his own name. Even if the file contains little or nothing
but blank paper, the air of familiarity with which the interrogator
refers to the subject's background can convince some sources that all
is known and that resistance is futile.
If the interrogatee is under detention, the interrogator can also
manipulate his environment. Merely by cutting off all other human contacts,
"the interrogator monopolizes the social environment of the source."(3)
He exercises the powers of an all-powerful parent, determining when
the source will be sent to bed, when and what he will eat, whether he
will be rewarded for good behavior or punished for being bad. The interrogator
can and does make the subject's world not only unlike the world to which
he had been accustomed but also strange in itself - a world in which
familiar patterns of time, space, and sensory perception are overthrown.
He can shift the environment abruptly. For example, a source who refuses
to talk at all can be placed in unpleasant solitary confinement for
a time. Then a friendly soul treats him to an unexpected walk in the
woods. Experiencing relief and exhilaration, the subject will usually
find it impossible not to respond to innocuous comments on the weather
and the flowers. These are expanded to include reminiscences, and soon
a precedent of verbal exchange has been established. Both the Germans
and the Chinese have used this trick effectively.
The interrogator also chooses the emotional key or keys in which the
interrogation or any part of it will be played.
Because of these and other advantages, " [approx. 6 lines deleted]
."(3)
Contents
B. The Structure of the Interrogation
A counterintelligence interrogation consists of four parts: the opening,
the reconnaissance, the detailed questioning and the conclusion.
Contents
1. The Opening
Most resistant interrogatees block off access to significant counterintelligence
in their possession for one or more of four reasons. The first is a
specific negative reaction to the interrogator. Poor initial handling
or a fundamental antipathy can make a source uncooperative even if he
has nothing significant or damaging to conceal. The second cause is
that some sources are resistant "by nature" - i.e. by early conditioning
- to any compliance with authority. The third is that the subject believes
that the information sought will be damaging or incriminating for him
personally that cooperation with the interrogator will have consequences
more painful for him than the results of non-cooperation. The fourth
is ideological resistance. The source has identified himself with a
cause, a political movement or organization, or an opposition intelligence
service. Regardless of his attitude toward the interrogator, his own
personality, and his fears for the future, the person who is deeply
devoted to a hostile cause will ordinarily prove strongly resistant
under interrogation.
A principal goal during the opening phase is to confirm the personality
assessment obtained through screening and to allow the interrogator
to gain a deeper understanding of the source as an individual. Unless
time is crucial, the interrogator should not become impatient if the
interrogatee wanders from the purposes of the interrogation and reverts
to personal concerns. Significant facts not produced during screening
may be revealed. The screening report itself is brought to life, the
type becomes an individual, as the subject talks. And sometimes seemingly
rambling monologues about personal matters are preludes to significant
admissions. Some people cannot bring themselves to provide information
that puts them in an unfavorable light until, through a lengthy prefatory
rationalization, they feel that they have set the stage that the interrogator
will now understand why they acted as they did. If face-saving is necessary
to the interrogatee it will be a waste of time to try to force him to
cut the preliminaries short and get down to cases. In his view, he is
dealing with the important topic, the why . He will be offended and
may become wholly uncooperative if faced with insistent demands for
the naked what .
There is another advantage in letting the subject talk freely and
even ramblingly in the first stage of interrogation. The interrogator
is free to observe. Human beings communicate a great deal by non-verbal
means. Skilled interrogators, for example, listen closely to voices
and learn a great deal from them. An interrogation is not merely a verbal
performance; it is a vocal performance, and the voice projects tension,
fear, a dislike of certain topics, and other useful pieces of information.
It is also helpful to watch the subject's mouth, which is as a rule
much more revealing than his eyes. Gestures and postures also tell a
story. If a subject normally gesticulates broadly at times and is at
other times physically relaxed but at some point sits stiffly motionless,
his posture is likely to be the physical image of his mental tension.
The interrogator should make a mental note of the topic that caused
such a reaction.
One textbook on interrogation lists the following physical indicators
of emotions and recommends that interrogators note them, not as conclusive
proofs but as assessment aids:
(1) A ruddy or flushed face is an indication of anger or embarrassment
but not necessarily of guilt.
(2) A "cold sweat" is a strong sign of fear and shock.
(3) A pale face indicates fear and usually shows that the interrogator
is hitting close to the mark.
(4) A dry mouth denotes nervousness.
(5) Nervous tension is also shown by wringing a handkerchief or clenching
the hands tightly.
(6) Emotional strain or tension may cause a pumping of the heart which
becomes visible in the pulse and throat.
(7) A slight gasp, holding the breath, or an unsteady voice may betray
the subject.
(8) Fidgeting may take many forms, all of which are good indications
of nervousness.
55 [page break]
(9) A man under emotional strain or nervous tension will involuntarily
draw his elbows to his sides. It is a protective defense mechanism.
(10) The movement of the foot when one leg is crossed over the knee
of the other can serve as an indicator. The circulation of the blood
to the lower leg is partially cut off, thereby causing a slight lift
or movement of the free foot with each heart beat. This becomes more
pronounced and observable as the pulse rate increases.
Pauses are also significant. Whenever a person is talking about a
subject of consequence to himself, he goes through a process of advance
self-monitoring, performed at lightning speed. This self-monitoring
is more intense if the person is talking to a stranger and especially
intense if he is answering the stranger's questions. Its purpose is
to keep from the questioner any guilty information or information that
would be damaging to the speaker's self-esteem. Where questions or answers
get close to sensitive areas, the pre-scanning is likely to create mental
blocks. These in turn produce unnatural pauses, meaningless sounds designed
to give the speaker more time, or other interruptions. It is not easy
to distinguish between innocent blocks -- things held back for reasons
of personal prestige -- and guilty blocks -- things the interrogator
needs to know. But the successful establishment of rapport will tend
to eliminate innocent blocks, or at least to keep them to a minimum.
The establishment of rapport is the second principal purpose of the
opening phase of the interrogation. Sometimes the interrogator knows
in advance, as a result of screening, that the subject will be uncooperative.
At other times the probability of resistance is established without
screening: detected hostile agents, for example, usually have not only
the will to resist but also the means, through a cover story or other
explanation. But the anticipation of withholding increases rather than
diminishes, the value of rapport. In other words, a lack of rapport
may cause an interrogatee to withhold information that he would otherwise
provide freely, whereas the existence of rapport may induce an interrogatee
who is initially determined to withhold to change his attitude. Therefore
the interrogator must not become hostile if confronted with initial
hostility, or in any other way confirm such negative attitudes as he
may encounter at the outset. During this first phase his attitude should
remain business-like but also quietly (not ostentatiously) friendly
and welcoming. Such opening remarks by subjects as, "I know what you
so-and-so's are after, and I can tell you right now that you're not
going to get it from me" are best handled by an unperturbed "Why don't
you tell me what has made you angry?" At this stage the interrogator
should avoid being drawn into conflict, no matter how provocatory may
be the attitude or language of the interrogatee. If he meets truculence
with neither insincere protestations that he is the subject's "pal"
nor an equal anger but rather a calm interest in what has aroused the
subject, the interrogator has gained two advantages right at the start.
He has established the superiority that he will need later, as the questioning
develops, and he has increased the chances of establishing rapport.
How long the opening phase continues depends upon how long it takes
to establish rapport or to determine that voluntary cooperation is unobtainable.
It may be literally a matter of seconds, or it may be a drawn-out, up-hill
battle. Even though the cost in time and patience is sometimes high,
the effort to make the subject feel that his questioner is a sympathetic
figure should not be abandoned until all reasonable resources have been
exhausted (unless, of course, the interrogation does not merit much
time). Otherwise, the chances are that the interrogation will not produce
optimum results. In fact, it is likely to be a failure, and the interrogator
should not be dissuaded from the effort to establish rapport by an inward
conviction that no man in his right mind would incriminate himself by
providing the kind of information that is sought. The history of interrogation
is full of confessions and other self-incriminations that were in essence
the result of a substitution of the interrogation world for the world
outside. In other words, as the sights and sounds of an outside world
fade away, its significance for the interrogatee tends to do likewise.
That world is replaced by the interrogation room, its two occupants,
and the dynamic relationship between them. As interrogation goes on,
the subject tends increasingly to divulge or withhold in accordance
with the values of the interrogation world rather than those of the
outside world (unless the periods of questioning are only brief interruptions
in his normal life). In this small world of two inhabitants a clash
of personalities -- as distinct from a conflict of purposes -- assumes
exaggerated force, like a tornado in a wind-tunnel. The self-esteem
of the interrogatee and of the interrogator becomes involved, and the
interrogatee fights to keep his secrets from his opponent for subjective
reasons, because he is grimly determined not to be the loser, the inferior.
If on the other hand the interrogator establishes rapport, the subject
may withhold because of other reasons, but his resistance often lacks
the bitter, last-ditch intensity that results if the contest becomes
personalized.
The interrogator who senses or determines in the opening phase that
what he is hearing is a legend should resist the first, natural impulse
to demonstrate its falsity. In some interrogatees the ego-demands, the
need to save face, are so intertwined with preservation of the cover
story that calling the man a liar will merely intensify resistance.
It is better to leave an avenue of escape, a loophole which permits
the source to correct his story without looking foolish.
If it is decided, much later in the interrogation, to confront the
interrogatee with proof of lying, the following related advice about
legal cross-examination may prove helpful.
"Much depends upon the sequence in which one conducts the cross-examination
of a dishonest witness. You should never hazard the important question
until you have laid the foundation for it in such a way that, when confronted
with the fact, the witness can neither deny nor explain it. One often
sees the most damaging documentary evidence, in the forms of letters
or affidavits, fall absolutely flat as betrayers of falsehood, merely
because of the unskillful way in which they are handled. If you have
in your possession a letter written by the witness, in which he takes
an opposite position on some part of the case to the one he has just
sworn to, avoid the common error of showing the witness the letter for
identification, and then reading it to him with the inquiry, 'What have
you to say to that?' During the reading of his letter the witness will
be collecting his thoughts and getting ready his explanations in anticipation
of the question that is to follow, and the effect of the damaging letter
will be lost.... The correct method of using such a letter is to lead
the witness quietly into repeating the statements he has made in his
direct testimony, and which his letter contradicts. Then read it off
to him. The witness has no explanation. He has stated the fact, there
is nothing to qualify."(41)
Contents
2. The Reconnaissance
If the interrogatee is cooperative at the outset or if rapport is
established during the opening phase and the source becomes cooperative,
the reconnaissance stage is needless; the interrogator proceeds directly
to detailed questioning. But if the interrogatee is withholding, a period
of exploration is necessary. Assumptions have normally been made already
as to what he is withholding: that he is a fabricator, or an RIS agent,
or something else he deems it important to conceal. Or the assumption
may be that he had knowledge of such activities carried out by someone
else. At any rate, the purpose of the reconnaissance is to provide a
quick testing of the assumption and, more importantly, to probe the
causes, extent, and intensity of resistance.
During the opening phase the interrogator will have charted the probable
areas of resistance by noting those topics which caused emotional or
physical reactions, speech blocks, or other indicators. He now begins
to probe these areas. Every experienced interrogator has noted that
if an interrogatee is withholding, his anxiety increases as the questioning
nears the mark. The safer the topic, the more voluble the source. But
as the questions make him increasingly uncomfortable, the interrogatee
becomes less communicative or perhaps even hostile. During the opening
phase the interrogator has gone along with this protective mechanism.
Now, however, he keeps coming back to each area of sensitivity until
he has determined the location of each and the intensity of the defenses.
If resistance is slight, mere persistence may overcome it; and detailed
questioning may follow immediately. But if resistance is strong, a new
topic should be introduced, and detailed questioning reserved for the
third stage.
Two dangers are especially likely to appear during the reconnaissance.
Up to this point the interrogator has not continued a line of questioning
when resistance was encountered. Now, however, he does so, and rapport
may be strained. Some interrogatees will take this change personally
and tend to personalize the conflict. The interrogator should resist
this tendency. If he succumbs to it, and becomes engaged in a battle
of wits, he may not be able to accomplish the task at hand. The second
temptation to avoid is the natural inclination to resort prematurely
to ruses or coercive techniques in order to settle the matter then and
there. The basic purpose of the reconnaissance is to determine the kind
and degree of pressure that will be needed in the third stage. The interrogator
should reserve his fire-power until he knows what he is up against.
Contents
3. The Detailed Questioning
a. If rapport is established and if the interrogatee has nothing significant
to hide, detailed questioning presents only routine problems. The major
routine considerations are the following:
The interrogator must know exactly what he wants to know. He should
have on paper or firmly in mind all the questions to which he seeks
answers. It usually happens that the source has a relatively large body
of information that has little or no intelligence value and only a small
collection of nuggets. He will naturally tend to talk about what he
knows best. The interrogator should not show quick impatience, but neither
should he allow the results to get out of focus. The determinant remains
what we need, not what the interrogatee can most readily provide.
At the same time it is necessary to make every effort to keep the
subject from learning through the interrogation process precisely where
our informational gaps lie. This principle is especially important if
the interrogatee is following his normal life, going home each evening
and appearing only once or twice a week for questioning, or if his bona
fides remains in doubt. Under almost all circumstances, however, a clear
revelation of our interests and knowledge should be avoided. It is usually
a poor practice to hand to even the most cooperative interrogatee an
orderly list of questions and ask him to write the answers. (This stricture
does not apply to the writing of autobiographies or on informational
matters not a subject of controversy with the source.) Some time is
normally spent on matters of little or no intelligence interest for
purposes of concealment. The interrogator can abet the process by making
occasional notes -- or pretending to do so -- on items that seem important
to the interrogatee but are not of intelligence value. From this point
of view an interrogation can be deemed successful if a source who is
actually a hostile agent can report to the opposition only the general
fields of our interest but cannot pinpoint specifics without including
misleading information.
It is sound practice to write up each interrogation report on the
day of questioning or, at least, before the next session, so that defects
can be promptly remedied and gaps or contradictions noted in time.
It is also a good expedient to have the interrogatee make notes of
topics that should be covered, which occur to him while discussing the
immediate matters at issue. The act of recording the stray item or thought
on paper fixes it in the interrogatee's mind. Usually topics popping
up in the course of an interrogation are forgotten if not noted; they
tend to disrupt the interrogation plan if covered by way of digression
on the spot.
Debriefing questions should usually be couched to provoke a positive
answer and should be specific. The questioner should not accept a blanket
negative without probing. For example, the question "Do you know anything
about Plant X?" is likelier to draw a negative answer then "Do you have
any friends who work at Plant X?" or "Can you describe its exterior?"
It is important to determine whether the subject's knowledge of any
topic was acquired at first hand, learned indirectly, or represents
merely an assumption. If the information was obtained indirectly, the
identities of sub-sources and related information about the channel
are needed. If statements rest on assumptions, the facts upon which
the conclusions are based are necessary to evaluation.
As detailed questioning proceeds, addition biographic data will be
revealed. Such items should be entered into the record, but it is normally
preferable not to diverge from an impersonal topic in order to follow
a biographic lead. Such leads can be taken up later unless they raise
new doubts about bona fides .
As detailed interrogation continues, and especially at the half-way
mark, the interrogator's desire to complete the task may cause him to
be increasingly business-like or even brusque. He may tend to curtail
or drop the usual inquiries about the subject's well-being with which
he opened earlier sessions. He may feel like dealing more and more abruptly
with reminiscences or digressions. His interest has shifted from the
interrogatee himself, who jut a while ago was an interesting person,
to the atsk of getting at what he knows. But if rapport has been established,
the interrogatee will be quick to sense and resent this change of attitude.
This point is particularly important if the interrogatee is a defector
faced with bewildering changes and in a highly emotional state. Any
interrogatee has his ups and downs, times when he is tired or half-ill,
times when his personal problems have left his nerves frayed. The peculiar
intimacy of the interrogation situation and the very fact that the interrogator
has deliberately fostered rapport will often lead the subject to talk
about his doubts, fears, and other personal reactions. The interrogator
should neither cut off this flow abruptly nor show impatience unless
it takes up an inordinate amount of time or unless it seems likely that
all the talking about personal matters is being used deliberately as
a smoke screen to keep the interrogator from doing his job. If the interrogatee
is believed cooperative, then from the beginning to the end of the process
he should feel that the interrogator's interest in him has remained
constant. Unless the interrogation is soon over, the interrogatee's
attitude toward his questioner is not likely to remain constant. He
will feel more and more drawn to the questioner or increasingly antagonistic.
As a rule, the best way for the interrogator to keep the relationship
on an even keel is to maintain the same quiet, relaxed, and open-minded
attitude from start to finish.
Detailed interrogation ends only when (1) all useful counterintelligence
information has been obtained; (2) diminishing returns and more pressing
commitments compel a cessation; or (3) the base, station, [one or two
words deleted] admits full or partial defeat. Termination for any reason
other than the first is only temporary. It is a profound mistake to
write off a successfully resistant interrogatee or one whose questioning
was ended before his potential was exhausted. KUBARK must keep track
of such persons, because people and circumstances change. Until the
source dies or tells us everything that he knows that is pertinent to
our purposes, his interrogation may be interrupted, perhaps for years
-- but it has not been completed.
Contents
4. The Conclusion
The end of an interrogation is not the end of the interrogator's responsibilities.
From the beginning of planning to the end of questioning it has been
necessary to understand and guard against the various troubles that
a vengeful ex-source can cause. As was pointed out earlier, KUBARK's
lack of executive authority abroad and its operational need for facelessness
make it peculiarly vulnerable to attack in the courts or the press.
The best defense against such attacks is prevention, through enlistment
or enforcement of compliance. However real cooperation is achieved,
its existence seems to act as a deterrent to later hostility. The initially
resistant subject may become cooperative because of a partial identification
with the interrogator and his interests, or the source may make such
an identification because of his cooperation. In either event, he is
unlikely to cause serious trouble in the future. Real difficulties are
more frequently created by interrogatees who have succeeded in withholding.
The following steps are normally a routine part of the conclusion:
a. [approx. 10 lines deleted]
d. [approx. 7 lines deleted]
e. [approx. 7 lines deleted]
f. [approx. 4 lines deleted]
Contents
C. Techniques of Non-Coercive Interrogation of
Resistant Sources
If source resistance is encountered during screening or during the
opening or reconnaissance phases of the interrogation, non-coercive
methods of sapping opposition and strengthening the tendency to yield
and to cooperate may be applied. Although these methods appear here
in an approximate order of increasing pressure, it should not be inferred
that each is to be tried until the key fits the lock. On the contrary,
a large part of the skill and the success of the experienced interrogator
lies in his ability to match method to source. The use of unsuccessful
techniques will of itself increase the interrogatee's will and ability
to resist.
This principle also affects the decision to employ coercive techniques
and governs the choice of these methods. If in the opinion of the interrogator
a totally resistant source has the skill and determination to withstand
any con-coercive method or combination of methods, it is better to avoid
them completely.
The effectiveness of most of the non-coercive techniques depends upon
their unsettling effect. The interrogation situation is in itself disturbing
to most people encountering it for the first time. The aim is to enhance
this effect, to disrupt radically the familiar emotional and psychological
associations of the subject. When this aim is achieved, resistance is
seriously impaired. There is an interval -- which may be extremely brief
-- of suspended animation, a kind of psychological shock or paralysis.
It is caused by a traumatic or sub-traumatic experience which explodes,
as it were, the world that is familiar to the subject as well as his
image of himself within that world. Experienced interrogators recognize
this effect when it appears and know that at this moment the source
is far more open to suggestion, far likelier to comply, than he was
just before he experienced the shock.
Another effect frequently produced by non-coercive (as well as coercive)
methods is the evocation within the interrogatee of feelings of guilt.
Most persons have areas of guilt in their emotional topographies, and
an interrogator can often chart these areas just by noting refusals
to follow certain lines of questioning. Whether the sense of guilt has
real or imaginary causes does not affect the result of intensification
of guilt feelings. Making a person feel more and more guilty normally
increases both his anxiety and his urge to cooperate as a means of escape.
In brief, the techniques that follow should match the personality
of the individual interrogatee, and their effectiveness is intensified
by good timing and rapid exploitation of the moment of shock. (A few
of the following items are drawn from Sheehan.) (32)
1. Going Next Door
Occasionally the information needed from a recalcitrant interrogatee
is obtainable from a willing source. The interrogator should decide
whether a confession is essential to his purpose or whether information
which may be held by others as well as the unwilling source is really
his goal. The labor of extracting the truth from unwilling interrogatees
should be undertaken only if the same information is not more easily
obtainable elsewhere or if operational considerations require self-incrimination.
2. Nobody Loves You
An interrogatee who is withholding items of no grave consequence to
himself may sometimes be persuaded to talk by the simple tactic of pointing
out that to date all of the information about his case has come from
persons other than himself. The interrogator wants to be fair. He recognizes
that some of the denouncers may have been biased or malicious. In any
case, there is bound to be some slanting of the facts unless the interrogatee
redresses the balance. The source owes it to himself to be sure that
the interrogator hears both sides of the story.
3. The All-Seeing Eye (or Confession is Good for the Soul)
The interrogator who already knows part of the story explains to the
source that the purpose of the questioning is not to gain information;
the interrogator knows everything already. His real purpose is to test
the sincerity (reliability, honor, etc.) of the source. The interrogator
then asks a few questions to which he knows the answers. If the subject
lies, he is informed firmly and dispassionately that he has lied. By
skilled manipulation of the known, the questioner can convince a naive
subject that all his secrets are out and that further resistance would
be not only pointless but dangerous. If this technique does not work
very quickly, it must be dropped before the interrogatee learns the
true limits of the questioner's knowledge.
4. The Informer
Detention makes a number of tricks possible. One of these, planting
an informant as the source's cellmate, is so well-known, especially
in Communist countries, that its usefulness is impaired if not destroyed.
Less well known is the trick of planting two informants in the cell.
One of them, A, tries now and then to pry a little information from
the source; B remains quiet. At the proper time, and during A's absence,
B warns the source not to tell A anything because B suspects him of
being an informant planted by the authorities.
Suspicion against a single cellmate may sometimes be broken down if
he shows the source a hidden microphone that he has "found" and suggests
that they talk only in whispers at the other end of the room.
5. News from Home
Allowing an interrogatee to receive carefully selected letters from
home can contribute to effects desired by the interrogator. Allowing
the source to write letters, especially if he can be led to believe
that they will be smuggled out without the knowledge of the authorities,
may produce information which is difficult to extract by direct questioning.
6. The Witness
If others have accused the interrogatee of spying for a hostile service
or of other activity which he denies, there is a temptation to confront
the recalcitrant source with his accuser or accusers. But a quick confrontation
has two weaknesses: it is likely to intensify the stubbornness of denials,
and it spoils the chance to use more subtle methods.
One of these is to place the interrogatee in an outer office and escort
past him, and into the inner office, an accuser whom he knows personally
or, in fact, any person -- even one who is friendly to the source and
uncooperative with the interrogators -- who is believed to know something
about whatever the interrogatee is concealing. It is also essential
that the interrogatee know or suspect that the witness may be in possession
of the incriminating information. The witness is whisked past the interrogatee;
the two are not allowed to speak to each other. A guard and a stenographer
remain in the outer office with the interrogatee. After about an hour
the interrogator who has been questioning the interrogatee in past sessions
opens the door and asks the stenographer to come in, with steno pad
and pencils. After a time she re-emerges and types material from her
pad, making several carbons. She pauses, points at the interrogatee,
and asks the guard how his name is spelled. She may also ask the interrogatee
directly for the proper spelling of a street, a prison, the name of
a Communist intelligence officer, or any other factor closely linked
to the activity of which he is accused. She takes her completed work
into the inner office, comes back out, and telephones a request that
someone come up to act as legal witness. Another man appears and enters
the inner office. The person cast in the informer's role may have been
let out a back door at the beginning of these proceedings; or if cooperative,
he may continue his role. In either event, a couple of interrogators,
with or without the "informer", now emerge from the inner office. In
contrast to their earlier demeanor, they are now relaxed and smiling.
The interrogator in charge says to the guard, "O.K., Tom, take him back.
We don't need him any more." Even if the interrogatee now insists on
telling his side of the story, he is told to relax, because the interrogator
will get around to him tomorrow or the next day.
A session with the witness may be recorded. If the witness denounces
the interrogatee there is no problem. If he does not, the interrogator
makes an effort to draw him out about a hostile agent recently convicted
in court or otherwise known to the witness. During the next interrogation
session with the source, a part of the taped denunciation can be played
back to him if necessary. Or the witnesses' remarks about the known
spy, edited as necessary, can be so played back that the interrogatee
is persuaded that he is the subject of the remarks.
Cooperative witnesses may be coached to exaggerate so that if a recording
is played for the interrogatee or a confrontation is arranged, the source
-- for example, a suspected courier -- finds the witness overstating
his importance. The witness claims that the interrogatee is only incidentally
a courier, that actually he is the head of an RIS kidnapping gang. The
interrogator pretends amazement and says into the recorder, "I thought
he was only a courier; and if he had told us the truth, I planned to
let him go. But this is much more serious. On the basis of charges like
these I'll have to hand him over to the local police for trial." On
hearing these remarks, the interrogatee may confess the truth about
the lesser guilt in order to avoid heavier punishment. If he continues
to withhold, the interrogator may take his side by stating, "You know,
I'm not at all convinced that so-and-so told a straight story. I feel,
personally, that he was exaggerating a great deal. Wasn't he? What's
the true story?"
7. Joint Suspects
If two or more interrogation sources are suspected of joint complicity
in acts directed against U.S. security, they should be separated immediately.
If time permits, it may be a good idea (depending upon the psychological
assessment of both) to postpone interrogation for about a week. Any
anxious inquiries from either can be met by a knowing grin and some
such reply as, "We'll get to you in due time. There's no hurry now ."
If documents, witnesses, or other sources yield information about interrogatee
A, such remarks as "B says it was in Smolensk that you denounced so-and-so
to the secret police. Is that right? Was it in 1937?" help to establish
in A's mind the impression that B is talking.
If the interrogator is quite certain of the facts in the case but
cannot secure an admission from either A or B, a written confession
may be prepared and A's signature may be reproduced on it. (It is helpful
if B can recognize A's signature, but not essential.) The confession
contains the salient facts, but they are distorted; the confession shows
that A is attempting to throw the entire responsibility upon B. Edited
tape recordings which sound as though A had denounced B may also be
used for the purpose, separately or in conjunction with the written
"confession." If A is feeling a little ill or dispirited, he can also
be led past a window or otherwise shown to B without creating a chance
for conversation; B is likely to interpret A's hang-dog look as evidence
of confession and denunciation. (It is important that in all such gambits,
A be the weaker of the two, emotionally and psychologically.) B then
reads (or hears) A's "confession." If B persists in withholding, the
interrogator should dismiss him promptly, saying that A's signed confession
is sufficient for the purpose and that it does not matter whether B
corroborates it or not. At the following session with B, the interrogator
selects some minor matter, not substantively damaging to B but nevertheless
exaggerated, and says, "I'm not sure A was really fair to you here.
Would you care to tell me your side of the story?" If B rises to this
bait, the interrogator moves on to areas of greater significance.
The outer-and-inner office routine may also be employed. A, the weaker,
is brought into the inner office, and the door is left slightly ajar
or the transom open. B is later brought into the outer office by a guard
and placed where he can hear, though not too clearly. The interrogator
begins routine questioning of A, speaking rather softly and inducing
A to follow suit. Another person in the inner office, acting by prearrangement,
then quietly leads A out through another door. Any noises of departure
are covered by the interrogator, who rattles the ash tray or moves a
table or large chair. As soon as the second door is closed again and
A is out of earshot, the interrogator resumes his questioning. His voice
grows louder and angrier. He tells A to speak up, that he can hardly
hear him. He grows abusive, reaches a climax, and then says, "Well,
that's better. Why didn't you say so in the first place?" The rest of
the monologue is designed to give B the impression that A has now started
to tell the truth. Suddenly the interrogator pops his head through the
doorway and is angry on seeing B and the guard. "You jerk!" he says
to the guard, "What are you doing here?" He rides down the guard's mumbled
attempt to explain the mistake, shouting, "Get him out of here! I'll
take care of you later!"
When, in the judgment of the interrogator, B is fairly well convinced
that A has broken down and told his story, the interrogator may elect
to say to B, "Now that A has come clean with us, I'd like to let him
go. But I hate to release one of you before the other; you ought to
get out at the same time. A seems to be pretty angry with you -- feels
that you got him into this jam. He might even go back to your Soviet
case officer and say that you haven't returned because you agreed to
stay here and work for us. Wouldn't it be better for you if I set you
both free together? Wouldn't it be better to tell me your side of the
story?"
8. Ivan Is a Dope
It may be useful to point out to a hostile agent that the cover story
was ill-contrived, that the other service botched the job, that it is
typical of the other service to ignore the welfare of its agents. The
interrogator may personalize this pitch by explaining that he has been
impressed by the agent's courage and intelligence. He sells the agent
the idea that the interrogator, not his old service, represents a true
friend, who understands him and will look after his welfare.
9. Joint Interrogators
The commonest of the joint interrogator techniques is the Mutt-and-Jeff
routine: the brutal, angry, domineering type contrasted with the friendly,
quiet type. This routine works best with women, teenagers, and timid
men. If the interrogator who has done the bulk of the questioning up
to this point has established a measure of rapport, he should play the
friendly role. If rapport is absent, and especially if antagonism has
developed, the principal interrogator may take the other part. The angry
interrogator speaks loudly from the beginning; and unless the interrogatee
clearly indicates that he is now ready to tell his story, the angry
interrogator shouts down his answers and cuts him off. He thumps the
table. The quiet interrogator should not watch the show unmoved but
give subtle indications that he too is somewhat afraid of his colleague.
The angry interrogator accuses the subject of other offenses, any offenses,
especially those that are heinous or demeaning. He makes it plain that
he personally considers the interrogatee the vilest person on earth.
During the harangue the friendly, quiet interrogator breaks in to say,
"Wait a minute, Jim. Take it easy." The angry interrogator shouts back,
"Shut up! I'm handling this. I've broken crumb-bums before, and I'll
break this one, wide open." He expresses his disgust by spitting on
the floor or holding his nose or any gross gesture. Finally, red-faced
and furious, he says, "I'm going to take a break, have a couple of stiff
drinks. But I'll be back at two -- and you, you bum, you better be ready
to talk." When the door slams behind him, the second interrogator tells
the subject how sorry he is, how he hates to work with a man like that
but has no choice, how if maybe brutes like that would keep quiet and
give a man a fair chance to tell his side of the story, etc., etc.
An interrogator working alone can also use the Mutt-and-Jeff technique.
After a number of tense and hostile sessions the interrogatee is ushered
into a different or refurnished room with comfortable furniture, cigarettes,
etc. The interrogator invites him to sit down and explains his regret
that the source's former stubbornness forced the interrogator to use
such tactics. Now everything will be different. The interrogator talks
man-to-man. An American POW, debriefed on his interrogation by a hostile
service that used this approach, has described the result: "Well, I
went in and there was a man, an officer he was... -- he asked me to
sit down and was very friendly.... It was very terrific. I, well, I
almost felt like I had a friend sitting there. I had to stop every now
and then and realize that this man wasn't a friend of mine.... I also
felt as though I couldn't be rude to him.... It was much more difficult
for me to -- well, I almost felt I had as much responsibility to talk
to him and reason and justification as I have to talk to you right now."(18)
Another joint technique casts both interrogators in friendly roles.
But whereas the interrogator in charge is sincere, the second interrogator's
manner and voice convey the impression that he is merely pretending
sympathy in order to trap the interrogated. He slips in a few trick
questions of the "When-did-you-stop-beating-your-wife?" category. The
interrogator in charge warns his colleague to desist. When he repeats
the tactics, the interrogator in charge says, with a slight show of
anger, "We're not here to trap people but to get at the truth. I suggest
that you leave now. I'll handle this."
It is usually unproductive to cast both interrogators in hostile roles.
Language
If the recalcitrant subject speaks more than one language, it is better
to question him in the tongue with which he is least familiar as long
as the purpose of interrogation is to obtain a confession. After the
interrogatee admits hostile intent or activity, a switch to the better-known
language will facilitate follow-up.
An abrupt switch of languages may trick a resistant source. If an
interrogatee has withstood a barrage of questions in German or Korean,
for example, a sudden shift to "Who is your case officer?" in Russian
may trigger the answer before the source can stop himself.
An interrogator quite at home in the language being used may nevertheless
elect to use an interpreter if the interrogatee does not know the language
to be used between the interrogator and interpreter and also does not
know that the interrogator knows his own tongue. The principal advantage
here is that hearing everything twice helps the interrogator to note
voice, expression, gestures, and other indicators more attentively.
This gambit is obviously unsuitable for any form of rapid-fire questioning,
and in any case it has the disadvantage of allowing the subject to pull
himself together after each query. It should be used only with an interpreter
who has been trained in the technique.
It is of basic importance that the interrogator not using an interpreter
be adept in the language selected for use. If he is not, if slips of
grammar or a strong accent mar his speech, the resistant source will
usually feel fortified. Almost all people have been conditioned to relate
verbal skill to intelligence, education, social status, etc. Errors
or mispronunciations also permit the interrogatee to misunderstand or
feign misunderstanding and thus gain time. He may also resort to polysyllabic
obfuscations upon realizing the limitations of the interrogator's vocabulary.
Spinoza and Mortimer Snerd
If there is reason to suspect that a withholding source possesses
useful counterintelligence information but has not had access to the
upper reaches of the target organizations, the policy and command level,
continued questioning about lofty topics that the source knows nothing
about may pave the way for the extraction of information at lower levels.
The interrogatee is asked about KGB policy, for example: the relation
of the service to its government, its liaison arrangements, etc., etc.
His complaints that he knows nothing of such matters are met by flat
insistence that he does know, he would have to know, that even the most
stupid men in his position know. Communist interrogators who used this
tactic against American POW's coupled it with punishment for "don't
know" responses -- typically by forcing the prisoner to stand at attention
until he gave some positive response. After the process had been continued
long enough, the source was asked a question to which he did know the
answer. Numbers of Americans have mentioned "...the tremendous feeling
of relief you get when he finally asks you something you can answer."
One said, "I know it seems strange now, but I was positively grateful
to them when they switched to a topic I knew something about."(3)
The Wolf in Sheep's Clothing
It has been suggested that a successfully withholding source might
be tricked into compliance if led to believe that he is dealing with
the opposition. The success of the ruse depends upon a successful imitation
of the opposition. A case officer previously unknown to the source and
skilled in the appropriate language talks with the source under such
circumstances that the latter is convinced that he is dealing with the
opposition. The source is debriefed on what he has told the Americans
and what he has not told them. The trick is likelier to succeed if the
interrogatee has not been in confinement but a staged "escape," engineered
by a stool-pigeon, might achieve the same end. Usually the trick is
so complicated and risky that its employment is not recommended.
Alice in Wonderland
The aim of the Alice in Wonderland or confusion technique is to confound
the expectations and conditioned reactions of the interrogatee. He is
accustomed to a world that makes some sense, at least to him: a world
of continuity and logic, a predictable world. He clings to this world
to reinforce his identity and powers of resistance.
The confusion technique is designed not only to obliterate the familiar
but to replace it with the weird. Although this method can be employed
by a single interrogator, it is better adapted to use by two or three.
When the subject enters the room, the first interrogator asks a doubletalk
question -- one which seems straightforward but is essentially nonsensical.
Whether the interrogatee tries to answer or not, the second interrogator
follows up (interrupting any attempted response) with a wholly unrelated
and equally illogical query. Sometimes two or more questions are asked
simultaneously. Pitch, tone, and volume of the interrogators' voices
are unrelated to the import of the questions. No pattern of questions
and answers is permitted to develop, nor do the questions themselves
relate logically to each other. In this strange atmosphere the subject
finds that the pattern of speech and thought which he has learned to
consider normal have been replaced by an eerie meaninglessness. The
interrogatee may start laughing or refuse to take the situation seriously.
But as the process continues, day after day if necessary, the subject
begins to try to make sense of the situation, which becomes mentally
intolerable. Now he is likely to make significant admissions, or even
to pour out his story, just to stop the flow of babble which assails
him. This technique may be especially effective with the orderly, obstinate
type.
Regression
There are a number of non-coercive techniques for inducing regression,
All depend upon the interrogator's control of the environment and, as
always, a proper matching of method to source. Some interrogatees can
be repressed by persistent manipulation of time, by retarding and advancing
clocks and serving meals at odd times -- ten minutes or ten hours after
the last food was given. Day and night are jumbled. Interrogation sessions
are similarly unpatterned the subject may be brought back for more questioning
just a few minutes after being dismissed for the night. Half-hearted
efforts to cooperate can be ignored, and conversely he can be rewarded
for non-cooperation. (For example, a successfully resisting source may
become distraught if given some reward for the "valuable contribution"
that he has made.) The Alice in Wonderland technique can reinforce the
effect. Two or more interrogators, questioning as a team and in relays
(and thoroughly jumbling the timing of both methods) can ask questions
which make it impossible for the interrogatee to give sensible, significant
answers. A subject who is cut off from the world he knows seeks to recreate
it, in some measure, in the new and strange environment. He may try
to keep track of time, to live in the familiar past, to cling to old
concepts of loyalty, to establish -- with one or more interrogators
-- interpersonal relations resembling those that he has had earlier
with other people, and to build other bridges back to the known. Thwarting
his attempts to do so is likely to drive him deeper and deeper into
himself, until he is no longer able to control his responses in adult
fashion.
The placebo technique is also used to induce regression The interrogatee
is given a placebo (a harmless sugar pill). Later he is told that he
has imbibed a drug, a truth serum, which will make him want to talk
and which will also prevent his lying. The subject's desire to find
an excuse for the compliance that represents his sole avenue of escape
from his distressing predicament may make him want to believe that he
has been drugged and that no one could blame him for telling his story
now. Gottschelk observes, "Individuals under increased stress are more
likely to respond to placebos."(7)
Orne has discussed an extensions of the placebo concept in explaining
what he terms the "magic room" technique. "An example... would be...
the prisoner who is given a hypnotic suggestion that his hand is growing
warm. However, in this instance, the prisoner's hand actually does become
warm, a problem easily resolved by the use of a concealed diathermy
machine. Or it might be suggested... that... a cigarette will taste
bitter. Here again, he could be given a cigarette prepared to have a
slight but noticeably bitter taste." In discussing states of heightened
suggestibility (which are not, however, states of trance) Orne says,
"Both hypnosis and some of the drugs inducing hypnoidal states are popularly
viewed as situations where the individual is no longer master of his
own fate and therefore not responsible for his actions. It seems possible
then that the hypnotic situation, as distinguished from hypnosis itself,
might be used to relieve the individual of a feeling of responsibility
for his own actions and thus lead him to reveal information."(7)
In other words, a psychologically immature source, or one who has
been regressed, could adopt an implication or suggestion that he has
been drugged, hypnotized, or otherwise rendered incapable of resistance,
even if he recognizes at some level that the suggestion is untrue, because
of his strong desire to escape the stress of the situation by capitulating.
These techniques provide the source with the rationalization that he
needs.
Whether regression occurs spontaneously under detention or interrogation,
and whether it is induced by a coercive or non-coercive technique, it
should not be allowed to continue past the point necessary to obtain
compliance. Severe techniques of regression are best employed in the
presence of a psychiatrist, to insure full reversal later. As soon as
he can, the interrogator presents the subject with the way out, the
face-saving reason for escaping from his painful dilemma by yielding.
Now the interrogator becomes fatherly. Whether the excuse is that others
have already confessed ("all the other boys are doing it"), that the
interrogatee had a chance to redeem himself ("you're really a good boy
at heart"), or that he can't help himself ("they made you do it"), the
effective rationalization, the one the source will jump at, is likely
to be elementary. It is an adult's version of the excuses of childhood.
The Polygraph
The polygraph can be used for purposes other than the evaluation of
veracity. For example, it may be used as an adjunct in testing the range
of languages spoken by an interrogatee or his sophistication in intelligence
matters, for rapid screening to determine broad areas of knowledgeability,
and as an aid in the psychological assessment of sources. Its primary
function in a counterintelligence interrogation, however, is to provide
a further means of testing for deception or withholding.
A resistant source suspected of association with a hostile clandestine
organization should be tested polygraphically at least once. Several
examinations may be needed. As a general rule, the polygraph should
not be employed as a measure of last resort. More reliable readings
will be obtained if the instrument is used before the subject has been
placed under intense pressure, whether such pressure is coercive or
not. Sufficient information for the purpose is normally available after
screening and one or two interrogation sessions.
Although the polygraph has been a valuable aid, no interrogator should
feel that it can carry his responsibility for him. [approx. 7 lines
deleted] (9)
The best results are obtained when the CI interrogator and the polygraph
operator work closely together in laying the groundwork for technical
examination. The operator needs all available information about the
personality of the source, as well as the operational background and
reasons for suspicion. The CI interrogator in turn can cooperate more
effectively and can fit the results of technical examination more accurately
into the totality of his findings if he has a basic comprehension of
the instrument and its workings.
The following discussion is based upon R.C. Davis' "Physiological
Responses as a Means of Evaluating Information."(7) Although improvements
appear to be in the offing, the instrument in widespread use today measures
breathing, systolic blood pressure, and galvanic skin response (GSR).
"One drawback in the use of respiration as an indicator," according
to Davis, "is its susceptibility to voluntary control." Moreover, if
the source "knows that changes in breathing will disturb all physiologic
variables under control of the autonomic division of the nervous system,
and possibly even some others, a certain amount of cooperation or a
certain degree of ignorance is required for lie detection by physiologic
methods to work." In general, "... breathing during deception is shallower
and slower than in truth telling... the inhibition of breathing seems
rather characteristic of anticipation of a stimulus."
The measurement of systolic blood pressure provides a reading on a
phenomenon not usually subject to voluntary control. The pressure "...
will typically rise by a few millimeters of mercury in response to a
question, whether it is answered truthfully or not. The evidence is
that the rise will generally be greater when (the subject) is lying."
However, discrimination between truth-telling and lying on the basis
of both breathing and blood pressure "... is poor (almost nil) in the
early part of the sitting and improves to a high point later."
The galvanic skin response is one of the most easily triggered reactions,
but recovery after the reaction is slow, and "... in a routine examination
the next question is likely to be introduced before recovery is complete.
Partly because of this fact there is an adapting trend in the GSR with
stimuli repeated every few minutes the response gets smaller, other
things being equal."
Davis examines three theories regarding the polygraph. The conditional
response theory holds that the subject reacts to questions that strike
sensitive areas, regardless of whether he is telling the truth or not.
Experimentation has not substantiated this theory. The theory of conflict
presumes that a large physiologic disturbance occurs when the subject
is caught between his habitual inclination to tell the truth and his
strong desire not to divulge a certain set of facts. Davis suggests
that if this concept is valid, it holds only if the conflict is intense.
The threat-of-punishment theory maintains that a large physiologic response
accompanies lying because the subject fears the consequence of failing
to deceive. "In common language it might be said that he fails to deceive
the machine operator for the very reason that he fears he will fail.
The 'fear' would be the very reaction detected." This third theory is
more widely held than the other two. Interrogators should note the inference
that a resistant source who does not fear that detection of lying will
result in a punishment of which he is afraid would not, according to
this theory, produce significant responses.
Graphology
The validity of graphological techniques for the analysis of the personalities
of resistant interrogatees has not been established. There is some evidence
that graphology is a useful aid in the early detection of cancer and
of certain mental illnesses. If the interrogator or his unit decides
to have a source's handwriting analyzed, the samples should be submitted
to Headquarters as soon as possible, because the analysis is more useful
in the preliminary assessment of the source than in the later interrogation.
Graphology does have the advantage of being one of the very few techniques
not requiring the assistance or even the awareness of the interrogatee.
As with any other aid, the interrogator is free to determine for himself
whether the analysis provides him with new and valid insights, confirms
other observations, is not helpful, or is misleading.
Contents
IX. Coercive Counterintelligence Interrogation
of Resistant Sources
A. Restrictions
The purpose of this part of the handbook is to present basic information
about coercive techniques available for use in the interrogation situation.
It is vital that this discussion not be misconstrued as constituting
authorization for the use of coercion at field discretion . As was noted
earlier, there is no such blanket authorization.
[approx. 10 lines deleted]
For both ethical and pragmatic reasons no interrogator may take upon
himself the unilateral responsibility for using coercive methods. Concealing
from the interrogator's superiors an intent to resort to coercion, or
its unapproved employment, does not protect them. It places them, and
KUBARK, in unconsidered jeopardy.
Contents
B. The Theory of Coercion
Coercive procedures are designed not only to exploit the resistant
source's internal conflicts and induce him to wrestle with himself but
also to bring a superior outside force to bear upon the subject's resistance.
Non-coercive methods are not likely to succeed if their selection and
use is not predicated upon an accurate psychological assessment of the
source. In contrast, the same coercive method may succeed against persons
who are very unlike each other. The changes of success rise steeply,
nevertheless, if the coercive technique is matched to the source's personality.
Individuals react differently even to such seemingly non-discriminatory
stimuli as drugs. Moreover, it is a waste of time and energy to apply
strong pressures on a hit-or-miss basis if a tap on the psychological
jugular will produce compliance.
All coercive techniques are designed to induce regression. As Hinkle
notes in "The Physiological State of the Interrogation Subject as it
Affects Brain Function"(7), the result of external pressures of sufficient
intensity is the loss of those defenses most recently acquired by civilized
man: "... the capacity to carry out the highest creative activities,
to meet new, challenging, and complex situations, to deal with trying
interpersonal relations, and to cope with repeated frustrations. Relatively
small degrees of homeostatic derangement, fatigue, pain, sleep loss,
or anxiety may impair these functions." As a result, "most people who
are exposed to coercive procedures will talk and usually reveal some
information that they might not have revealed otherwise."
One subjective reaction often evoked by coercion is a feeling of guilt.
Meltzer observes, "In some lengthy interrogations, the interrogator
may, by virtue of his role as the sole supplier of satisfaction and
punishment, assume the stature and importance of a parental figure in
the prisoner's feeling and thinking. Although there may be intense hatred
for the interrogator, it is not unusual for warm feelings also to develop.
This ambivalence is the basis for guilt reactions, and if the interrogator
nourishes these feelings, the guilt may be strong enough to influence
the prisoner's behavior.... Guilt makes compliance more likely...."(7).
Farber says that the response to coercion typically contains "...
at least three important elements: debility, dependency, and dread."
Prisoners "... have reduced viability, are helplessly dependent on their
captors for the satisfaction of their many basic needs, and experience
the emotional and motivational reactions of intense fear and anxiety....
Among the [American] POW's pressured by the Chinese Communists, the
DDD syndrome in its full-blown form constituted a state of discomfort
that was well-nigh intolerable." (11). If the debility-dependency-dread
state is unduly prolonged, however, the arrestee may sink into a defensive
apathy from which it is hard to arouse him.
Psychologists and others who write about physical or psychological
duress frequently object that under sufficient pressure subjects usually
yield but that their ability to recall and communicate information accurately
is as impaired as the will to resist. This pragmatic objection has somewhat
the same validity for a counterintelligence interrogation as for any
other. But there is one significant difference. Confession is a necessary
prelude to the CI interrogation of a hitherto unresponsive or concealing
source. And the use of coercive techniques will rarely or never confuse
an interrogatee so completely that he does not know whether his own
confession is true or false. He does not need full mastery of all his
powers of resistance and discrimination to know whether he is a spy
or not. Only subjects who have reached a point where they are under
delusions are likely to make false confessions that they believe. Once
a true confession is obtained, the classic cautions apply. The pressures
are lifted, at least enough so that the subject can provide counterintelligence
information as accurately as possible. In fact, the relief granted the
subject at this time fits neatly into the interrogation plan. He is
told that the changed treatment is a reward for truthfulness and an
evidence that friendly handling will continue as long as he cooperates.
The profound moral objection to applying duress past the point of
irreversible psychological damage has been stated. Judging the validity
of other ethical arguments about coercion exceeds the scope of this
paper. What is fully clear, however, is that controlled coercive manipulation
of an interrogatee may impair his ability to make fine distinctions
but will not alter his ability to answer correctly such gross questions
as "Are you a Soviet agent? What is your assignment now? Who is your
present case officer?"
When an interrogator senses that the subject's resistance is wavering,
that his desire to yield is growing stronger than his wish to continue
his resistance, the time has come to provide him with the acceptable
rationalization: a face-saving reason or excuse for compliance. Novice
interrogators may be tempted to seize upon the initial yielding triumphantly
and to personalize the victory. Such a temptation must be rejected immediately.
An interrogation is not a game played by two people, one to become the
winner and the other the loser. It is simply a method of obtaining correct
and useful information. Therefore the interrogator should intensify
the subject's desire to cease struggling by showing him how he can do
so without seeming to abandon principle, self-protection, or other initial
causes of resistance. If, instead of providing the right rationalization
at the right time, the interrogator seizes gloatingly upon the subject's
wavering, opposition will stiffen again.
The following are the principal coercive techniques of interrogation:
arrest, detention, deprivation of sensory stimuli through solitary confinement
or similar methods, threats and fear, debility, pain, heightened suggestibility
and hypnosis, narcosis, and induced regression. This section also discusses
the detection of malingering by interrogatees and the provision of appropriate
rationalizations for capitulating and cooperating.
Contents
C. Arrest
The manner and timing of arrest can contribute substantially to the
interrogator's purposes. "What we aim to do is to ensure that the manner
of arrest achieves, if possible, surprise, and the maximum amount of
mental discomfort in order to catch the suspect off balance and to deprive
him of the initiative. One should therefore arrest him at a moment when
he least expects it and when his mental and physical resistance is at
its lowest. The ideal time at which to arrest a person is in the early
hours of the morning because surprise is achieved then, and because
a person's resistance physiologically as well as psychologically is
at its lowest.... If a person cannot be arrested in the early hours...,
then the next best time is in the evening....
[approx. 10 lines deleted]" (1)
Contents
D. Detention
If, through the cooperation of a liaison service or by unilateral
means, arrangements have been made for the confinement of a resistant
source, the circumstances of detention are arranged to enhance within
the subject his feelings of being cut off from the known and the reassuring,
and of being plunged into the strange. Usually his own clothes are immediately
taken away, because familiar clothing reinforces identity and thus the
capacity for resistance. (Prisons give close hair cuts and issue prison
garb for the same reason.) If the interrogatee is especially proud or
neat, it may be useful to give him an outfit that is one or two sizes
too large and to fail to provide a belt, so that he must hold his pants
up.
The point is that man's sense of identity depends upon a continuity
in his surroundings, habits, appearance, actions, relations with others,
etc. Detention permits the interrogator to cut through these links and
throw the interrogatee back upon his own unaided internal resources.
Little is gained if confinement merely replaces one routine with another.
Prisoners who lead monotonously unvaried lives "... cease to care about
their utterances, dress, and cleanliness. They become dulled, apathetic,
and depressed."(7) And apathy can be a very effective defense against
interrogation. Control of the source's environment permits the interrogator
to determine his diet, sleep pattern, and other fundamentals. Manipulating
these into irregularities, so that the subject becomes disorientated,
is very likely to create feelings of fear and helplessness. Hinkle points
out, "People who enter prison with attitudes of foreboding, apprehension,
and helplessness generally do less well than those who enter with assurance
and a conviction that they can deal with anything that they may encounter....
Some people who are afraid of losing sleep, or who do not wish to lose
sleep, soon succumb to sleep loss...." (7)
In short, the prisoner should not be provided a routine to which he
can adapt and from which he can draw some comfort -- or at least a sense
of his own identity. Everyone has read of prisoners who were reluctant
to leave their cells after prolonged incarceration. Little is known
about the duration of confinement calculated to make a subject shift
from anxiety, coupled with a desire for sensory stimuli and human companionship,
to a passive, apathetic acceptance of isolation and an ultimate pleasure
in this negative state. Undoubtedly the rate of change is determined
almost entirely by the psychological characteristics of the individual.
In any event, it is advisable to keep the subject upset by constant
disruptions of patterns.
For this reason, it is useful to determine whether the interrogattee
has been jailed before, how often, under what circumstances, for how
long, and whether he was subjected to earlier interrogation. Familiarity
with confinement and even with isolation reduces the effect.
Contents
E. Deprivation of Sensory Stimuli
The chief effect of arrest and detention, and particularly of solitary
confinement, is to deprive the subject of many or most of the sights,
sounds, tastes, smells, and tactile sensations to which he has grown
accustomed. John C. Lilly examined eighteen autobiographical accounts
written by polar explorers and solitary seafarers. He found "... that
isolation per se acts on most persons as a powerful stress.... In all
cases of survivors of isolation at sea or in the polar night, it was
the first exposure which caused the greatest fears and hence the greatest
danger of giving way to symptoms; previous experience is a powerful
aid in going ahead, despite the symptoms. "The symptoms most commonly
produced by isolation are superstition, intense love of any other living
thing, perceiving inanimate objects as alive, hallucinations, and delusions."
(26)
The apparent reason for these effects is that a person cut off from
external stimuli turns his awareness inward, upon himself, and then
projects the contents of his own unconscious outwards, so that he endows
his faceless environment with his own attributes, fears, and forgotten
memories. Lilly notes, "It is obvious that inner factors in the mind
tend to be projected outward, that some of the mind's activity which
is usually reality-bound now becomes free to turn to phantasy and ultimately
to hallucination and delusion."
A number of experiments conducted at McGill University, the National
Institute of Mental Health, and other sites have attempted to come as
close as possible to the elimination of sensory stimuli, or to masking
remaining stimuli, chiefly sounds, by a stronger but wholly monotonous
overlay. The results of these experiments have little applicability
to interrogation because the circumstances are dissimilar. Some of the
findings point toward hypotheses that seem relevant to interrogation,
but conditions like those of detention for purposes of counterintelligence
interrogation have not been duplicated for experimentation.
At the National Institute of Mental Health two subjects were "...
suspended with the body and all but the top of the head immersed in
a tank containing slowly flowing water at 34.5 [degrees] C (94.5 [degrees]
F)...." Both subjects wore black-out masks, which enclosed the whole
head but allowed breathing and nothing else. The sound level was extremely
low; the subject heard only his own breathing and some faint sounds
of water from the piping. Neither subject stayed in the tank longer
than three hours. Both passed quickly from normally directed thinking
through a tension resulting from unsatisfied hunger for sensory stimuli
and concentration upon the few available sensations to private reveries
and fantasies and eventually to visual imagery somewhat resembling hallucinations.
"In our experiments, we notice that after immersion the day apparently
is started over, i. e., the subject feels as if he has risen from bed
afresh; this effect persists, and the subject finds he is out of step
with the clock for the rest of the day."
Drs. Wexler, Mendelson, Leiderman, and Solomon conducted a somewhat
similar experiment on seventeen paid volunteers. These subjects were
"... placed in a tank-type respirator with a specially built mattress....
The vents of the respirator were left open, so that the subject breathed
for himself. His arms and legs were enclosed in comfortable but rigid
cylinders to inhibit movement and tactile contact. The subject lay on
his back and was unable to see any part of his body. The motor of the
respirator was run constantly, producing a dull, repetitive auditory
stimulus. The room admitted no natural light, and artificial light was
minimal and constant." (42) Although the established time limit was
36 hours and though all physical needs were taken care of, only 6 of
the 17 completed the stint. The other eleven soon asked for release.
Four of these terminated the experiment because of anxiety and panic;
seven did so because of physical discomfort. The results confirmed earlier
findings that (1) the deprivation of sensory stimuli induces stress;
(2) the stress becomes unbearable for most subjects; (3) the subject
has a growing need for physical and social stimuli; and (4) some subjects
progressively lose touch with reality, focus inwardly, and produce delusions,
hallucinations, and other pathological effects.
In summarizing some scientific reporting on sensory and perceptual
deprivation, Kubzansky offers the following observations:
"Three studies suggest that the more well-adjusted or 'normal' the
subject is, the more he is affected by deprivation of sensory stimuli.
Neurotic and psychotic subjects are either comparatively unaffected
or show decreases in anxiety, hallucinations, etc." (7)
These findings suggest - but by no means prove - the following theories
about solitary confinement and isolation:
1. The more completely the place of confinement eliminates sensory
stimuli, the more rapidly and deeply will the interrogatee be affected.
Results produced only after weeks or months of imprisonment in an ordinary
cell can be duplicated in hours or days in a cell which has no light
(or weak artificial light which never varies), which is sound-proofed,
in which odors are eliminated, etc. An environment still more subject
to control, such as water-tank or iron lung, is even more effective.
2. An early effect of such an environment is anxiety. How soon it
appears and how strong it is depends upon the psychological characteristics
of the individual.
3. The interrogator can benefit from the subject's anxiety. As the
interrogator becomes linked in the subject's mind with the reward of
lessened anxiety, human contact, and meaningful activity, and thus with
providing relief for growing discomfort, the questioner assumes a benevolent
role. (7)
4. The deprivation of stimuli induces regression by depriving the
subject's mind of contact with an outer world and thus forcing it in
upon itself. At the same time, the calculated provision of stimuli during
interrogation tends to make the regressed subject view the interrogator
as a father-figure. The result, normally, is a strengthening of the
subject's tendencies toward compliance.
Contents
F. Threats and Fear
The threat of coercion usually weakens or destroys resistance more
effectively than coercion itself. The threat to inflict pain, for example,
can trigger fears more damaging than the immediate sensation of pain.
In fact, most people underestimate their capacity to withstand pain.
The same principle holds for other fears: sustained long enough, a strong
fear of anything vague or unknown induces regression, whereas the materialization
of the fear, the infliction of some form of punishment, is likely to
come as a relief. The subject finds that he can hold out, and his resistances
are strengthened. "In general, direct physical brutality creates only
resentment, hostility, and further defiance." (18)
The effectiveness of a threat depends not only on what sort of person
the interrogatee is and whether he believes that his questioner can
and will carry the threat out but also on the interrogator's reasons
for threatening. If the interrogator threatens because he is angry,
the subject frequently senses the fear of failure underlying the anger
and is strengthened in his own resolve to resist. Threats delivered
coldly are more effective than those shouted in rage. It is especially
important that a threat not be uttered in response to the interrogatee's
own expressions of hostility. These, if ignored, can induce feelings
of guilt, whereas retorts in kind relieve the subject's feelings.
Another reason why threats induce compliance not evoked by the inflection
of duress is that the threat grants the interrogatee time for compliance.
It is not enough that a resistant source should placed under the tension
of fear; he must also discern an acceptable escape route. Biderman observes,
"Not only can the shame or guilt of defeat in the encounter with the
interrogator be involved, but also the more fundamental injunction to
protect one's self-autonomy or 'will'.... A simple defense against threats
to the self from the anticipation of being forced to comply is, of course,
to comply 'deliberately' or 'voluntarily'.... To the extent that the
foregoing interpretation holds, the more intensely motivated the [interrogatee]
is to resist, the more intense is the pressure toward early compliance
from such anxieties, for the greater is the threat to self-esteem which
is involved in contemplating the possibility of being 'forced to' comply...."
(6) In brief, the threat is like all other coercive techniques in being
most effective when so used as to foster regression and when joined
with a suggested way out of the dilemma, a rationalization acceptable
to the interrogatee.
The threat of death has often been found to be worse than useless.
It "has the highest position in law as a defense, but in many interrogation
situations it is a highly ineffective threat. Many prisoners, in fact,
have refused to yield in the face of such threats who have subsequently
been 'broken' by other procedures." (3) The principal reason is that
the ultimate threat is likely to induce sheer hopelessness if the interrogatee
does not believe that it is a trick; he feels that he is as likely to
be condemned after compliance as before. The threat of death is also
ineffective when used against hard-headed types who realize that silencing
them forever would defeat the interrogator's purpose. If the threat
is recognized as a bluff, it will not only fail but also pave the way
to failure for later coercive ruses used by the interrogator.
Contents
G. Debility
No report of scientific investigation of the effect of debility upon
the interrogatee's powers of resistance has been discovered. For centuries
interrogators have employed various methods of inducing physical weakness:
prolonged constraint; prolonged exertion; extremes of heat, cold, or
moisture; and deprivation or drastic reduction of food or sleep. Apparently
the assumption is that lowering the source's physiological resistance
will lower his psychological capacity for opposition. If this notion
were valid, however, it might reasonably be expected that those subjects
who are physically weakest at the beginning of an interrogation would
be the quickest to capitulate, a concept not supported by experience.
The available evidence suggests that resistance is sapped principally
by psychological rather than physical pressures. The threat of debility
- for example, a brief deprivation of food - may induce much more anxiety
than prolonged hunger, which will result after a while in apathy and,
perhaps, eventual delusions or hallucinations. In brief, it appears
probable that the techniques of inducing debility become counter-productive
at an early stage. The discomfort, tension, and restless search for
an avenue of escape are followed by withdrawal symptoms, a turning away
from external stimuli, and a sluggish unresponsiveness.
Another objection to the deliberate inducing of debility is that prolonged
exertion, loss of sleep, etc., themselves become patterns to which the
subject adjusts through apathy. The interrogator should use his power
over the resistant subject's physical environment to disrupt patterns
of response, not to create them. Meals and sleep granted irregularly,
in more than abundance or less than adequacy, the shifts occuring on
no discernible time pattern, will normally disorient an interrogatee
and sap his will to resist more effectively than a sustained deprivation
leading to debility.
Contents
H. Pain
Everyone is aware that people react very differently to pain. The
reason, apparently, is not a physical difference in the intensity of
the sensation itself. Lawrence E. Hinkle observes, "The sensation of
pain seems to be roughly equal in all men, that is to say, all people
have approximately the same threshold at which they begin to feel pain,
and when carefully graded stimuli are applied to them, their estimates
of severity are approximately the same.... Yet... when men are very
highly motivated... they have been known to carry out rather complex
tasks while enduring the most intense pain." He also states, "In general,
it appears that whatever may be the role of the constitutional endowment
in determining the reaction to pain, it is a much less important determinant
than is the attitude of the man who experiences the pain." (7)
The wide range of individual reactions to pain may be partially explicable
in terms of early conditioning. The person whose first encounters with
pain were frightening and intense may be more violently affected by
its later infliction than one whose original experiences were mild.
Or the reverse may be true, and the man whose childhood familiarized
him with pain may dread it less, and react less, than one whose distress
is heightened by fear of the unknown. The individual remains the determinant.
It has been plausibly suggested that, whereas pain inflicted on a
person from outside himself may actually focus or intensify his will
to resist, his resistance is likelier to be sapped by pain which he
seems to inflict upon himself. "In the simple torture situation the
contest is one between the individual and his tormentor (.... and he
can frequently endure). When the individual is told to stand at attention
for long periods, an intervening factor is introduced. The immediate
source of pain is not the interrogator but the victim himself. The motivational
strength of the individual is likely to exhaust itself in this internal
encounter.... As long as the subject remains standing, he is attributing
to his captor the power to do something worse to him, but there is actually
no showdown of the ability of the interrogator to do so." (4)
Interrogatees who are withholding but who feel qualms of guilt and
a secret desire to yield are likely to become intractable if made to
endure pain. The reason is that they can then interpret the pain as
punishment and hence as expiation. There are also persons who enjoy
pain and its anticipation and who will keep back information that they
might otherwise divulge if they are given reason to expect that withholding
will result in the punishment that they want. Persons of considerable
moral or intellectual stature often find in pain inflicted by others
a confirmation of the belief that they are in the hands of inferiors,
and their resolve not to submit is strengthened.
Intense pain is quite likely to produce false confessions, concocted
as a means of escaping from distress. A time-consuming delay results,
while investigation is conducted and the admissions are proven untrue.
During this respite the interrogatee can pull himself together. He may
even use the time to think up new, more complex "admissions" that take
still longer to disprove. KUBARK is especially vulnerable to such tactics
because the interrogation is conducted for the sake of information and
not for police purposes.
If an interrogatee is caused to suffer pain rather late in the interrogation
process and after other tactics have failed, he is almost certain to
conclude that the interrogator is becoming desperate. He may then decide
that if he can just hold out against this final assault, he will win
the struggle and his freedom. And he is likely to be right. Interrogatees
who have withstood pain are more difficult to handle by other methods.
The effect has been not to repress the subject but to restore his confidence
and maturity.
Contents
I. Heightened Suggestibility and Hypnosis
In recent years a number of hypotheses about hypnosis have been advanced
by psychologists and others in the guise of proven principles. Among
these are the flat assertions that a person connot be hypnotized against
his will; that while hypnotized he cannot be induced to divulge information
that he wants urgently to conceal; and that he will not undertake, in
trance or through post-hypnotic suggestion, actions to which he would
normally have serious moral or ethical objections. If these and related
contentions were proven valid, hypnosis would have scant value for the
interrogator.
But despite the fact that hypnosis has been an object of scientific
inquiry for a very long time, none of these theories has yet been tested
adequately. Each of them is in conflict with some observations of fact.
In any event, an interrogation handbook cannot and need not include
a lengthy discussion of hypnosis. The case officer or interrogator needs
to know enough about the subject to understand the circumstances under
which hypnosis can be a useful tool, so that he can request expert assistance
appropriately.
Operational personnel, including interrogators, who chance to have
some lay experience or skill in hypnotism should not themselves use
hypnotic techniques for interrogation or other operational purposes.
There are two reasons for this position. The first is that hypnotism
used as an operational tool by a practitioner who is not a psychologist,
psychiatrist, or M.D. can produce irreversible psychological damage.
The lay practitioner does not know enough to use the technique safely.
The second reason is that an unsuccessful attempt to hypnotize a subject
for purposes of interrogation, or a successful attempt not adequately
covered by post-hypnotic amnesia or other protection, can easily lead
to lurid and embarrassing publicity or legal charges.
Hypnosis is frequently called a state of heightened suggestibility,
but the phrase is a description rather than a definition. Merton M.
Gill and Margaret Brenman state, "The psychoanalytic theory of hypnosis
clearly implies, where it does not explicitly state, that hypnosis is
a form of regression." And they add, "...induction [of hypnosis] is
the process of bringing about a regression, while the hypnotic state
is the established regression." (13) It is suggested that the interrogator
will find this definition the most useful. The problem of overcoming
the resistance of an uncooperative interrogatee is essentially a problem
of inducing regression to a level at which the resistance can no longer
be sustained. Hypnosis is one way of regressing people.
Martin T. Orne has written at some length about hypnosis and interrogation.
Almost all of his conclusions are tentatively negative. Concerning the
role played by the will or attitude of the interrogates, Orne says,
"Although the crucial experiment has not yet been done, there is little
or no evidence to indicate that trance can be induced against a person's
wishes." He adds, "...the actual occurrence of the trance state is related
to the wish of the subject to enter hypnosis." And he also observes,
"...whether a subject will or will not enter trance depends upon his
relationship with the hyponotist rather than upon the technical procedure
of trance induction." These views are probably representative of those
of many psychologists, but they are not definitive. As Orne himself
later points out, the interrogatee "... could be given a hypnotic drug
with appropriate verbal suggestions to talk about a given topic. Eventually
enough of the drug would be given to cause a short period of unconsciousness.
When the subject wakes, the interrogator could then read from his 'notes'
of the hypnotic interview the information presumably told him." (Orne
had previously pointed out that this technique requires that the interrogator
possess significant information about the subject without the subject's
knowledge.) "It can readily be seen how this... maneuver... would facilitate
the elicitation of information in subsequent interviews." (7) Techniques
of inducing trance in resistant subjects through preliminary administration
of so-called silent drugs (drugs which the subject does not know he
has taken) or through other non-routine methods of induction are still
under investigation. Until more facts are known, the question of whether
a resister can be hypnotized involuntarily must go unanswered.
Orne also holds that even if a resister can be hypnotized, his resistance
does not cease. He postulates "... that only in rare interrogation subjects
would a sufficiently deep trance be obtainable to even attempt to induce
the subject to discuss material which he is unwilling to discuss in
the waking state. The kind of information which can be obtained in these
rare instances is still an unanswered question." He adds that it is
doubtful that a subject in trance could be made to reveal information
which he wished to safeguard. But here too Orne seems somewhat too cautious
or pessimistic. Once an interrogatee is in a hypnotic trance, his understanding
of reality becomes subject to manipulation. For example, a KUBARK interrogator
could tell a suspect double agent in trance that the KGB is conducting
the questioning, and thus invert the whole frame of reference. In other
words, Orne is probably right in holding that most recalcitrant subjects
will continue effective resistance as long as the frame of reference
is undisturbed. But once the subject is tricked into believing that
he is talking to friend rather than foe, or that divulging the truth
is the best way to serve his own purposes, his resistance will be replaced
by cooperation. The value of hypnotic trance is not that it permits
the interrogator to impose his will but rather that it can be used to
convince the interrogatee that there is no valid reason not to be forthcoming.
A third objection raised by Orne and others is that material elicited
during trance is not reliable. Orne says, "... it has been shown that
the accuracy of such information... would not be guaranteed since subjects
in hypnosis are fully capable of lying." Again, the observation is correct;
no known manipulative method guarantees veracity. But if hypnosis is
employed not as an immediate instrument for digging out the truth but
rather as a way of making the subject want to align himself with his
interrogators, the objection evaporates.
Hypnosis offers one advantage not inherent in other interrogation
techniques or aids: the post-hypnotic suggestion. Under favorable circumstances
it should be possible to administer a silent drug to a resistant source,
persuade him as the drug takes effect that he is slipping into a hypnotic
trance, place him under actual hypnosis as consciousness is returning,
shift his frame of reference so that his reasons for resistance become
reasons for cooperating, interrogate him, and conclude the session by
implanting the suggestion that when he emerges from trance he will not
remember anything about what has happened.
This sketchy outline of possible uses of hypnosis in the interrogation
of resistant sources has no higher goal than to remind operational personnel
that the technique may provide the answer to a problem not otherwise
soluble. To repeat: hypnosis is distinctly not a do-it-yourself project.
Therefore the interrogator, base, or center that is considering its
use must anticipate the timing sufficiently not only to secure the obligatory
headquarters permission but also to allow for an expert's travel time
and briefing.
Contents
J. Narcosis
Just as the threat of pain may more effectively induce compliance
than its infliction, so an interrogatee's mistaken belief that he has
been drugged may make him a more useful interrogation subject than he
would be under narcosis. Louis A. Gottschalk cites a group of studies
as indicating "that 30 to 50 per cent of individuals are placebo reactors,
that is, respond with symptomatic relief to taking an inert substance."
(7) In the interrogation situation, moreover, the effectiveness of a
placebo may be enhanced because of its ability to placate the conscience.
The subject's primary source of resistance to confession or divulgence
may be pride, patriotism, personal loyalty to superiors, or fear of
retribution if he is returned to their hands. Under such circumstances
his natural desire to escape from stress by complying with the interrogator's
wishes may become decisive if he is provided an acceptable rationalization
for compliance. "I was drugged" is one of the best excuses.
Drugs are no more the answer to the interrogator's prayer than the
polygraph, hypnosis, or other aids. Studies and reports "dealing with
the validity of material extracted from reluctant informants... indicate
that there is no drug which can force every informant to report all
the information he has. Not only may the inveterate criminal psychopath
lie under the influence of drugs which have been tested, but the relatively
normal and well-adjusted individual may also successfully disguise factual
data." (3) Gottschalk reinforces the latter observation in mentioning
an experiment involving drugs which indicated that "the more normal,
well-integrated individuals could lie better than the guilt-ridden,
neurotic subjects." (7)
Nevertheless, drugs can be effective in overcoming resistance not
dissolved by other techniques. As has already been noted, the so-called
silent drug (a pharmacologically potent substance given to a person
unaware of its administration) can make possible the induction of hypnotic
trance in a previously unwilling subject. Gottschalk says, "The judicious
choice of a drug with minimal side effects, its matching to the subject's
personality, careful gauging of dosage, and a sense of timing... [make]
silent administration a hard-to-equal ally for the hypnotist intent
on producing self-fulfilling and inescapable suggestions... the drug
effects should prove... compelling to the subject since the perceived
sensations originate entirely within himself." (7)
Particularly important is the reference to matching the drug to the
personality of the interrogatee. The effect of most drugs depends more
upon the personality of the subject than upon the physical characteristics
of the drugs themselves. If the approval of Headquarters has been obtained
and if a doctor is at hand for administration, one of the most important
of the interrogator's functions is providing the doctor with a full
and accurate description of the psychological make-up of the interrogatee,
to facilitate the best possible choice of a drug.
Persons burdened with feelings of shame or guilt are likely to unburden
themselves when drugged, especially if these feelings have been reinforced
by the interrogator. And like the placebo, the drug provides an excellent
rationalization of helplessness for the interrogatee who wants to yield
but has hitherto been unable to violate his own values or loyalties.
Like other coercive media, drugs may affect the content of what an
interrogatee divulges. Gottschalk notes that certain drugs "may give
rise to psychotic manifestations such as hallucinations, illusions,
delusions, or disorientation", so that "the verbal material obtained
cannot always be considered valid." (7) For this reason drugs (and the
other aids discussed in this section) should not be used persistently
to facilitate the interrogative debriefing that follows capitulation.
Their function is to cause capitulation, to aid in the shift from resistance
to cooperation. Once this shift has been accomplished, coercive techniques
should be abandoned both for moral reasons and because they are unnecessary
and even counter-productive.
This discussion does not include a list of drugs that have been employed
for interrogation purposes or a discussion of their properties because
these are medical considerations within the province of a doctor rather
than an interogator.
Contents
K. The Detection of Malingering
The detection of malingering is obviously not an interrogation technique,
coercive or otherwise. But the history of interrogation is studded with
the stories of persons who have attempted, often successfully, to evade
the mounting pressures of interrogation by feigning physical or mental
illness. KUBARK interrogators may encounter seemingly sick or irrational
interrogatees at times and places which make it difficult or next-to-impossible
to summon medical or other professional assistance. Because a few tips
may make it possible for the interrogator to distinguish between the
malingerer and the person who is genuinely ill, and because both illness
and malingering are sometimes produced by coercive interrogation, a
brief discussion of the topic has been included here.
Most persons who feign a mental or physical illness do not know enough
about it to deceive the well-informed. Malcolm L. Meltzer says, "The
detection of malingering depends to a great extent on the simulator's
failure to understand adequately the characteristics of the role he
is feigning.... Often he presents symptoms which are exceedingly rare,
existing mainly in the fancy of the layman. One such symptom is the
delusion of misidentification, characterized by the... belief that he
is some powerful or historic personage. This symptom is very unusual
in true psychosis, but is used by a number of simulators. In schizophrenia,
the onset tends to be gradual, delusions do not spring up full-blown
over night; in simulated disorders, the onset is usually fast and delusions
may be readily available. The feigned psychosis often contains many
contradictory and inconsistent symptoms, rarely existing together. The
malingerer tends to go to extremes in his portrayal of his symptoms;
he exaggerates, overdramatizes, grimaces, shouts, is overly bizarre,
and calls attention to himself in other ways....
"Another characteristic of the malingerer is that he will usually
seek to evade or postpone examination. A study of the behavior of lie-detector
subjects, for example, showed that persons later 'proven guilty' showed
certain similarities of behavior. The guilty persons were reluctant
to take the test, and they tried in various ways to postpone or delay
it. They often appeared highly anxious and sometimes took a hostile
attitude toward the test and the examiner. Evasive tactics sometimes
appeared, such as sighing, yawning, moving about, all of which foil
the examiner by obscuring the recording. Before the examination, they
felt it necessary to explain why their responses might mislead the examiner
into thinking they were lying. Thus the procedure of subjecting a suspected
malingerer to a lie-detector test might evoke behavior which would reinforce
the suspicion of fraud." (7)
Meltzer also notes that malingerers who are not professional psychologists
can usually be exposed through Rorschach tests.
An important element in malingering is the frame of mind of the examiner.
A person pretending madness awakens in a professional examiner not only
suspicion but also a desire to expose the fraud, whereas a well person
who pretends to be concealing mental illness and who permits only a
minor symptom or two to peep through is much likelier to create in the
expert a desire to expose the hidden sickness.
Meltzer observes that simulated mutism and amnesia can usually be
distinguished from the true states by narcoanalysis. The reason, however,
is the reverse of the popular misconception. Under the influence of
appropriate drugs the malingerer will persist in not speaking or in
not remembering, whereas the symptoms of the genuinely afflicted will
temporarily disappear. Another technique is to pretend to take the deception
seriously, express grave concern, and tell the "patient" that the only
remedy for his illness is a series of electric shock treatments or a
frontal lobotomy.
Contents
L. Conclusion
A brief summary of the foregoing may help to pull the major concepts
of coercive interrogation together:
1. The principal coercive techniques are arrest, detention, the deprivation
of sensory stimuli, threats and fear, debility, pain, heightened suggestibility
and hypnosis, and drugs.
2. If a coercive technique is to be used, or if two or more are to
be employed jointly, they should be chosen for their effect upon the
individual and carefully selected to match his personality.
3. The usual effect of coercion is regression. The interrogatee's
mature defenses crumbles as he becomes more childlike. During the process
of regression the subject may experience feelings of guilt, and it is
usually useful to intensify these.
4. When regression has proceeded far enough so that the subject's
desire to yield begins to overbalance his resistance, the interrogator
should supply a face-saving rationalization. Like the coercive technique,
the rationalization must be carefully chosen to fit the subject's personality.
5. The pressures of duress should be slackened or lifted after compliance
has been obtained, so that the interrogatee's voluntary cooperation
will not be impeded.
No mention has been made of what is frequently the last step in an
interrogation conducted by a Communist service: the attempted conversion.
In the Western view the goal of the questioning is information; once
a sufficient degree of cooperation has been obtained to permit the interrogator
access to the information he seeks, he is not ordinarily concerned with
the attitudes of the source. Under some circumstances, however, this
pragmatic indifference can be short-sighted. If the interrogatee remains
semi-hostile or remorseful after a successful interrogation has ended,
less time may be required to complete his conversion (and conceivably
to create an enduring asset) than might be needed to deal with his antagonism
if he is merely squeezed and forgotten.
Contents
X. Interrogator's Check List
The questions that follow are intended as reminders for the interrogator
and his superiors.
1. Have local (federal or other) laws affecting KUBARK's conduct of
a unilateral or joint interrogation been compiled and learned?
2. If the interrogatee is to be held, how long may he be legally detained?
3. Are interrogations conducted by other ODYOKE departments and agencies
with foreign counterintelligence responsibilities being coordinated
with KUBARK if subject to the provisions of Chief/KUBARK Directive [one-word
deletion] or Chief/KUBARK Directive [one-word deletion] ? Has a planned
KUBARK interrogation subject to the same provisions been appropriately
coordinated?
4. Have applicable KUBARK regulations and directives been observed?
These include [approx. 1/2 line deleted], the related Chief/KUBARK Directives,
[approx. 1/2 line deleted] pertinent [one or two words deleted], and
the provisions governing duress which appear in various paragraphs of
this handbook.
5. Is the prospective interrogatee a PBPRIME citizen? If so, have
the added considerations listed on various paragraphs been duly noted?
6. Does the interrogators selected for the task meet the four criteria
of (a) adequate training and experience, (b) genuine familiarity with
the language to be used, (c) knowledge of the geographical/cultural
area concerned, and (d) psychological comprehension of the interrogatee?
7. Has the prospective interrogatee been screened? What are his major
psychological characteristics? Does he belong to one of the nine major
categories listed in pp. 19-28? Which?
8. Has all available and pertinent information about the subject been
assembled and studied?
9. Is the source [approx. 2/3 line deleted], or will questioning be
completed elsewhere? If at a base or station, will the interrogator,
interrogatee, and facilities be available for the time estimated as
necessary to the completion of the process? If he is to be sent to a
center, has the approval of the center or of Headquarters been obtained?
10. Have all appropriate documents carried by the prospective interrogatee
been subjected to technical analysis?
11. Has a check of logical overt sources been conducted? Is the interrogation
necessary?
12. Have field and headquarters traces been run on the potential interrogatee
and persons closely associated with him by emotional, family, or business
ties?
13. Has a preliminary assessment of bona fides been carried out? With
what results?
14. If an admission of prior association with one or more foreign
intelligence services or Communist parties or fronts has been obtained,
have full particulars been acquired and reported?
15. Has LCFLUTTER been administered? As early as practicable? More
than once? When?
16. Is it estimated that the prospective interrogatee is likely to
prove cooperative or recalcitrant? If resistance is expected, what is
its anticipated source: fear, patriotism, personal considerations, political
convictions, stubbornness, other?
17. What is the purpose of the interrogation?
18. Has an interrogation plan been prepared?
19. [approx. 5 lines deleted]
20. Is an appropriate setting for interrogation available?
21. Will the interrogation sessions be recorded? Is the equipment
available? Installed?
22. Have arrangements been made to feed, bed, and guard the subject
as necessary?
23. Does the interrogation plan call for more than one interrogator?
If so, have roles been assigned and schedules prepared?
24. Is the interrogational environment fully subject to the interrogator's
manipulation and control?
25. What disposition is planned for the interrogatee after the questioning
ends?
26. Is it possible, early in the questioning, to determine the subject's
personal response to the interrogator or interrogators? What is the
interrogator's reaction to the subject? Is there an emotional reaction
strong enough to distort results? If so, can the interrogator be replaced?
27. If the source is resistant, will noncoercive or coercive techniques
be used? What is the reason for the choice?
28. Has the subject been interrogated earlier? Is he sophisticated
about interrogation techniques?
29. Does the impression made by the interrogatee during the opening
phase of the interrogation confirm or conflict with the preliminary
assessment formed before interrogation started? If there are significant
differences, what are they and how do they affect the plan for the remainder
of the questioning?
30. During the opening phase, have the subject's voice, eyes, mouth,
gestures, silences, or other visible clues suggested areas of sensitivity?
If so, on what topics?
31. Has rapport been established during the opening phase?
32. Has the opening phase been followed by a reconnaissance? What
are the key areas of resistance? What tactics and how much pressure
will be required to overcome the resistance? Should the estimated duration
of interrogation be revised? If so, are further arrangements necessary
for continued detention, liaison support, guarding, or other purposes?
33. In the view of the interrogator, what is the emotional reaction
of the subject to the interrogator? Why?
34. Are interrogation reports being prepared after each session, from
notes or tapes?
35. What disposition of the interrogatee is to be made after questioning
ends? If the subject is suspected of being a hostile agent and if interrogation
has not produced confession, what measures will be taken to ensure that
he is not left to operate as before, unhindered and unchecked?
36. Are any promises made to the interrogatee unfulfilled when questioning
ends? Is the subject vengeful? Likely to try to strike back? How?
37. If one or more of the non-coercive techniques discussed on pp.
52-81 have been selected for use, how do they match the subject's personality?
38. Are coercive techniques to be employed? If so, have all field
personnel in the interrogator's direct chain of command been notified?
Have they approved?
39. Has prior Headquarters permission been obtained?
40. [approx. 4 lines deleted]
41. As above, for confinement. If the interrogates is to be confined,
can KUBARK control his environment fully? Can the normal routines be
disrupted for interrogation purposes?
42. Is solitary confinement to be used? Why? Does the place of confinement
permit the practical elimination of sensory stimuli?
43. Are threats to be employed? As part of a plan? Has the nature
of the threat been matched to that of the interrogatee?
44. If hypnosis or drugs are thought necessary, has Headquarters been
given enough advance notice? Has adequate allowance been made for travel
time and other preliminaries?
45. Is the interrogatee suspected of malingering? If the interrogator
is uncertain, are the services of an expert available?
46. At the conclusion of the interrogation, has a comprehensive summary
report been prepared?
47. [approx. 4 lines deleted]
48. [approx. 4 lines deleted]
49. Was the interrogation a success? Why?
50. A failure? Why?
Contents
XI. Descriptive Bibliography
This bibliography is selective; most of the books and articles consulted
during the preparation of this study have not been included here. Those
that have no real bearing on the counterintelligence interrogation of
resistant sources have been left out. Also omitted are some sources
considered elementary, inferior, or unsound. It is not claimed that
what remains is comprehensive as well as selective, for the number of
published works having some relevance even to the restricted subject
is over a thousand. But it is believed that all the items listed here
merit reading by KUBARK personnel concerned with interrogation.
1. Anonymous [approx. 1/3 line deleted], Interrogation , undated.
This paper is a one-hour lecture on the subject. It is thoughtful, forthright,
and based on extensive experience. It deals only with interrogation
following arrest and detention. Because the scope is nevertheless broad,
the discussion is brisk but necessarily less than profound.
2. Barioux, Max, "A Method for the Selection, Training, and Evaluation
of Interviewers," Public Opinion Quarterly , Spring 1952, Vol. 16, No.
1. This article deals with the problems of interviewers conducting public
opinion polls. It is of only slight value for interrogators, although
it does suggest pitfalls produced by asking questions that suggest their
own answers.
3. Biderman, Albert D., A Study for Development of Improved Interrogation
Techniques : Study SR 177-D (U), Secret, final report of Contract AS
18 (600) 1797, Bureau of Social Science Research Inc., Washington, D.
C., March 1959. Although this book (207 pages of text) is principally
concerned with lessons derived from the interrogation of American POW's
by Communist services and with the problem of resisting interrogation,
it also deals with the interrogation of resistant subjects. It has the
added advantage of incorporating the findings and views of a number
of scholars and specialists in subjects closely related to interrogation.
As the frequency of citation indicates, this book was one of the most
useful works consulted; few KUBARK interrogators would fail to profit
from reading it. It also contains a descriminating but undescribed bibliography
of 343 items.
4. Biderman, Albert D., "Communist Attempts to Elicit False Confession
from Air Force Prisoners of War", Bulletin of the New York Academy of
Medicine , September 1957, Vol. 33. An excellent analysis of the psychological
pressures applied by Chinese Communists to American POW's to extract
"confessions" for propaganda purposes.
5. Biderman, Albert D., "Communist Techniques of Coercive Interrogation",
Air Intelligence , July 1955, Vol. 8, No. 7. This short article does
not discuss details. Its subject is closely related to that of item
4 above; but the focus is on interrogation rather than the elicitation
of "confessions".
6. Biderman, Albert D., "Social Psychological Needs and 'Involuntary'
Behavior as Illustrated by Compliance in Interrogation", Sociometry
, June 1960, Vol. 23. This interesting article is directly relevant.
It provides a useful insight into the interaction between interrogator
and interrogatee. It should be compared with Melton W. Horowitz's "Psychology
of Confession" (see below).
7. Biderman, Albert D. and Herbert Zimmer, The Manipulation of Human
Behavior , John Wiley and Sons Inc., New York and London, 1961. This
book of 304 pages consists of an introduction by the editors and seven
chapters by the following specialists: Dr. Lawrence E. Hinkle Jr., "The
Physiological State of the Interrogation Subject as it Affects Brain
Function"; Dr. Philip E. Kubzansky, "The Effects of Reduced Environmental
Stimulation on Human Behavior: A Review"; Dr. Louis A. Gottschalk, "The
Use of Drugs in Interrogation"; Dr. R. C. Davis, "Physiological Responses
as a Means of Evaluating Information" (this chapter deals with the polygraph);
Dr. Martin T. Orne, "The Potential Uses of Hypnosis In Interrogation";
Drs. Robert R. Blake and Jane S. Mouton, "The Experimental Investigation
of Interpersonal Influence"; and Dr. Malcolm L. Meltzer, "Countermanipulation
through Malingering." Despite the editors preliminary announcement that
the book has "a particular frame of reference; the interrogation of
an unwilling subject", the stress is on the listed psychological specialties;
and interrogation gets comparitively short shrift. Nevertheless, the
KUBARK interrogator should read this book, especially the chapters by
Drs. Orne and Meltzer. He will find that the book is by scientists for
scientists and that the contributions consistently demonstrate too theoretical
an understanding of interrogation per se. He will also find that practically
no valid experimentation the results of which were unclassified and
available to the authors has been conducted under interrogation conditions.
Conclusions are suggested, almost invariably, on a basis of extrapolation.
But the book does contain much useful information, as frequent references
in this study show. The combined bibliographies contain a total of 771
items.
8. [approx. 14 lines deleted]
10. [approx. 9 lines deleted]
11. [approx. 3 lines deleted]
[approx. 3 lines deleted]
12. [approx. 9 lines deleted]
13. Gill, Merton, Inc., and Margaret Brenman, Hypnosis and Related
States: Psychoanalytic Studies in Regression , International Universities
Press Inc., New York, 1959. This book is a scholarly and comprehensive
examination of hypnosis. The approach is basically Freudian but the
authors are neither narrow nor doctrinaire. The book discusses the induction
of hypnosis, the hypnotic state, theories of induction and of the hypnotic
condition, the concept of regression as a basic element in hypnosis,
relationships between hypnosis and drugs, sleep, fugue, etc., and the
use of hypnosis in psychotherapy. Interrogators may find the comparison
between hypnosis and "brainwashing" in chapter 9 more relevant than
other parts. The book is recommended, however, not because it contains
any discussion of the employment of hypnosis in interrogation (it does
not) but because it provides the interrogator with sound information
about what hypnosis can and cannot do.
14. Hinkle, Lawrence E. Jr. and Harold G. Wolff, "Communist Interrogation
and Indoctrination of Enemies of the State", AMA Archives of Neurology
and Psychiatry , August 1956, Vol. 76, No. 2. This article summarizes
the physiological and psychological reactions of American prisoners
to Communist detention and interrogation. It merits reading but not
study, chiefly because of the vast differences between Communist interrogation
of American POW's and KUBARK interrogation of known or suspected personnel
of Communist services or parties.
15. Horowitz, Milton W., "Psychology of Confession." Journal of Criminal
Law, Criminology, and Police Science , July-August 1956, Vol. 47. The
author lists the following principles of confession: (1) the subject
feels accused; (2) he is confronted by authority wielding power greater
than his own; (3) he believes that evidence damaging to him is available
to or possessed by the authority; (4) the accused is cut off from friendly
support; (5) self-hostility is generated; and (6) confession to authority
promises relief. Although the article is essentially a speculation rather
than a report of verified facts, it merits close reading.
16. Inbau, Fred E. and John E. Reid, Lie Detection and Criminal Investigation
, Williams and Wilkin Co., 1953. The first part of this book consists
of a discussion of the polygraph. It will be more useful to the KUBARK
interrogator than the second, which deals with the elements of criminal
interrogation.
17. KHOKHLOV, Nicolai, In the Name of Conscience , David McKay Co.,
New York, 1959. This entry is included chiefly because of the cited
quotation. It does provide, however, some interesting insights into
the attitudes of an interrogatee.
18. KUBARK, Communist Control Methods , Appendix 1: "The Use of Scientific
Design and Guidance Drugs and Hypnosis in Communist Interrogation and
Indoctrination Procedures." Secret, no date. The appendix reports a
study of whether Communist interrogation methods included such aids
as hypnosis and drugs. Although experimentation in these areas is, of
course, conducted in Communist countries, the study found no evidence
that such methods are used in Communist interrogations -- or that they
would be necessary.
19. KUBARK (KUSODA), Communist Control Techniques , Secret, 2 April
1956. This study is an analysis of the methods used by Communist State
police in the arrest, interrogation, and indoctrination of persons regarded
as enemies of the state. This paper, like others which deal with Communist
interrogation techniques, may be useful to any KUBARK interrogator charged
with questioning a former member of an Orbit intelligence or security
service but does not deal with interrogation conducted without police
powers.
20. KUBARK, Hostile Control and Interrogation Techniques , Secret,
undated. This paper consists of 28 pages and two annexes. It provides
counsel to KUBARK personnel on how to resist interrogation conducted
by a hostile service. Although it includes advice on resistance, it
does not present any new information about the theories or practices
of interrogation.
21. [approx. 15 lines deleted]
23. Laycock, Keith, "Handwriting Analysis as an Assessment Aid," Studies
in Intelligence, Summer 1959, Vol. 3, No. 3. A defense of graphology
by an "educated amateur." Although the article is interesting, it does
not present tested evidence that the analysis of a subject's handwriting
would be a useful aid to an interrogator. Recommended, nevertheless,
for interrogators unfamiliar with the subject.
24. Lefton, Robert Jay, "Chinese Communist 'Thought Reform.': Confession
and Reeducation of Western Civilians," Bulletin of the New York Academy
of Medicine , September 1957, Vol. 33. A sound article about Chicom
brainwashing techniques. The information was compiled from first-hand
interviews with prisoners who had been subjected to the process. Recommended
as background reading.
25. Levenson, Bernard and Lee Wiggins, A Guide for Intelligence Interviewing
of Voluntary Foreign Sources , Official Use Only, Officer Education
Research Laboratory, ARDC, Maxwell Air Force Base (Technical Memorandum
OERL-TM-54-4.) A good, though generalized, treatise on interviewing
techniques. As the title shows, the subject is different from that of
the present study.
26. Lilly, John C., "Mental Effects of Reduction of Ordinary Levels
of Physical Stimuli on Intact Healthy Persons." Psychological Research
Report #5 , American Psychiatric Association, 1956. After presenting
a short summary of a few autobiographical accounts written about relative
isolation at sea (in small boats) or polar regions, the author describes
two experiments designed to mask or drastically reduce most sensory
stimulation. The effect was to speed up the results of the more usual
sort of isolation (for example, solitary confinement). Delusions and
hallucinations, preceded by other symptoms, appeared after short periods.
The author does not discuss the possible relevance of his findings to
interrogation.
27. Meerlo, Joost A.M., The Rape of the Mind , World Publishing Co.,
Cleveland, 1956. This book's primary value for the interrogator is that
it will make him aware of a number of elements in the responses of an
interrogatee which are not directly related to the questions asked or
the interrogation setting but are instead the product of (or are at
least influenced by) all questioning that the subject has undergone
earlier, especially as a child. For many interrogatees the interrogator
becomes, for better or worse, the parent or authority symbol. Whether
the subject is submissive or belligerent may be determined in part by
his childhood relationships with his parents. Because the same forces
are at work in the interrogator, the interrogation may be chiefly a
cover for a deeper layer of exchange or conflict between the two. For
the interrogator a primary value of this book (and of much related psychological
and psychoanalytic work) is that it may give him a deeper insight into
himself.
28. Moloney, James Clark, "Psychic Self-Abandon and Extortion of Confessions,"
International Journal of Psychoanalysis , January/February 1955, Vol.
36. This short article relates the psychological release obtained through
confession (i. e., the sense of well-being following surrender as a
solution to an otherwise unsolvable conflict) with religious experience
generally and some ten Buddhistic practices particularly. The interrogator
will find little here that is not more helpfully discussed in other
sources, including Gill and Brenman's Hypnosis and Related States .
Marginal.
29. Oatis, William N. "Why I Confessed," Life , 21 September 1953,
Vol. 35. Of some marginal value because it combines the writer's profession
of innocence ("I am not a spy and never was") with an account of how
he was brought to "confess" to espionage within three days of his arrest.
Although Oatis was periodically deprived of sleep (once for 42 hours)
and forced to stand until weary, the Czechs obtained the "confessions"
without torture or starvation and without sophisticated techniques.
30. Rundquist, E.A., "The Assessment of Graphology, " Studies in Intelligence
, Secret, Summer 1959, Vol. 3, No. 3. The author concludes that scientific
testing of graphology is needed to permit an objective assessment of
the claims made in its behalf. This article should be read in conjunction
with No. 23, above.
31. Schachter, Stanley, The Psychology of Affiliation: Experimental
Studies of the Sources of Gregariousness , Stanford University Press,
Stanford, California, 1959. A report of 133 pages, chiefly concerned
with experiments and statistical analyses performed at the University
of Minnesota by Dr. Schachter and colleagues. The principal findings
concern relationships among anxiety, strength of affiliative tendencies,
and the ordinal position (i.e., rank in birth sequence among siblings).
Some tentative conclusions of significance for interrogators are reached,
the following among them:
a. "One of the consequences of isolation appears to be a psychological
state which in its extreme form resembles a full-blown anxiety attack."
(p. 12.)
b. Anxiety increases the desire to be with others who share the same
fear.
c. Persons who are first-born or only children are typically more
nervous or afraid than those born later. Firstborns and onlies are also
"considerably less willing or able to withstand pain than are later-born
children." (p. 49.) In brief, this book presents hypotheses of interest
to interrogators but much further research is needed to test validity
and applicability.
32. Sheehan, Robert, Police Interview and Interrogations and the Preparation
and Signing of Statements . A 23-page pamphlet, unclassified and undated,
that discusses some techniques and tricks that can be used in counterintelligence
interrogation. The style is sprightly, but most of the material is only
slightly related to KUBARK's interrogation problems. Recommended as
background reading.
33. Singer, Margaret Thaler and Edgar H. Schein, "Projective Test
Responses of Prisoners of War Following Repatriation." Psychiatry ,
1958, Vol. 21. Tests conducted on American ex-POW's returned during
the Big and Little Switches in Korea showed differences in characteristics
between non-collaborators and corroborators. The latter showed more
typical and humanly responsive reactions to psychological testing than
the former, who tended to be more apathetic and emotionally barren or
withdrawn. Active resisters, however, often showed a pattern of reaction
or responsiveness like that of collaborators. Rorschach tests provided
clues, with a good statistical incidence of reliability, for differentiation
between collaborators and non-collaborators. The tests and results described
are worth noting in conjunction with the screening procedures recommended
in this paper.
34. Sullivan, Harry Stack, The Psychiatric Interview , W. W. Norton
and Co., New York, 1954. Any interrogator reading this book will be
struck by parallels between the psychiatric interview and the interrogation.
The book is also valuable because the author, a psychiatrist of considerable
repute, obviously had a deep understanding of the nature of the inter-personal
relationship and of resistance.
35. U.S. Army, Office of the Chief of Military History, Russian Methods
of Interrogating Captured Personnel in World War II , Secret, Washington,
1951. A comprehensive treatise on Russian intelligence and police systems
and on the history of Russian treatment of captives, military and civilian,
during and following World War II. The appendix contains some specific
case summaries of physical torture by the secret police. Only a small
part of the book deals with interrogation. Background reading.
36. U.S. Army, 7707 European Command Intelligence Center, Guide for
Intelligence Interrogators of Eastern Cases , Secret, April 1958. This
specialized study is of some marginal value for KUBARK interrogators
dealing with Russians and other Slavs.
37. U. S. Army, The Army Intelligence School, Fort Holabird, Techniques
of Interrogation , Instructors Folder I-6437/A, January 1956. This folder
consists largely of an article, "Without Torture," by a German ex-interrogator,
Hans Joachim Scharff. Both the preliminary discussion and the Scharff
article (first published in Argosy , May 1950) are exclusively concerned
with the interrogation of POW's. Although Scharff claims that the methods
used by German Military Intelligence against captured U.S. Air Force
personnel "... were almost irresistible," the basic technique consisted
of impressing upon the prisoner the false conviction that his information
was already known to the Germans in full detail. The success of this
method depends upon circumstances that are usually lacking in the peacetime
interrogation of a staff or agent member of a hostile intelligence service.
The article merits reading, nevertheless, because it shows vividly the
advantages that result from good planning and organization.
38. U. S. Army, Counterintelligence Corps, Fort Holabird, Interrogations,
Restricted, 5 September 1952. Basic coverage of military interrogation.
Among the subjects discussed are the interrogation of witnesses, suspects,
POW's, and refugees, and the employment of interpreters and of the polygraph.
Although this text does not concentrate upon the basic problems confronting
KUBARK interrogators, it will repay reading.
39. U.S. Army, Counterintelligence Corps, Fort Holabird, Investigative
Subjects Department, Interrogations, Restricted, 1 May 1950. This 70-gage
booklet on counterintelligence interrogation is basic, succinct, practical,
and sound. Recommended for close reading.
40. [approx. 5 lines deleted]
41. Wellman, Francis L., The Art of Cross-Examination , Garden City
Publishing Co. (now Doubleday), New York, originally 1903, 4th edition,
1948. Most of this book is but indirectly related to the subject of
this study; it is primarily concerned with tripping up witnesses and
impressing juries. Chapter VIII, "Fallacies of Testimony," is worth
reading, however, because some of its warnings are applicable.
42. Wexler, Donald, Jack Mendelson, Herbert Leiderman, and Philip
Solomon, "Sensory Deprivation," A.M.A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry
, 1958, 79, pp. 225-233. This article reports an experiment designed
to test the results of eliminating most sensory stimuli and masking
others. Paid volunteers spent periods from 1 hour and 38 minutes to
36 hours in a tank-respirator. The results included inability to concentrate
effectively, daydreaming and fantasy, illusions, delusions, and hallucinations.
The suitability of this procedure as a means of speeding up the effects
of solitary confinement upon recalcitrant subjects has not been considered.
OTHER BIBLIOGRAPHIES
The following bibliographies on interrogation were noted during the
preparation of this study.
1. Brainwashing, A Guide to the Literature , prepared by the Society
for the Investigation of Human Ecology, Inc., Forest Hills, New York,
December 1960. A wide variety of materials is represented: scholarly
and scientific reports, governmental and organizational reports, legal
discussions, biographical accounts, fiction, journalism, and miscellaneous.
The number of items in each category is, respectively, 139, 28, 7, 75,
10, 14, and 19, a total of 418. One or two sentence descriptions follow
the titles. These are restricted to an indication of content and do
not express value judgements. The first section contains a number of
especially useful references.
2. Comprehensive Bibliography of Interrogation Techniques, Procedures,
and Experiences , Air Intelligence Information Report, Unclassified,
10 June 1959. This bibliography of 158 items dating between 1915 and
1957 comprises "the monographs on this subject available in the Library
of Congress and arranged in alphabetical order by author, or in the
absence of an author, by title." No descriptions are included, except
for explanatory sub-titles. The monographs, in several languages, are
not categorized. This collection is extremely heterogeneous. Most of
the items are of scant or peripheral value to the interrogator.
3. Interrogation Methods and Techniques , KUPALM, L-3, 024, 941, July
1959, Secret/NOFORN. This bibliography of 114 items includes references
to four categories: books and pamphlets, articles from periodicals,
classified documents, and materials from classified periodicals. No
descriptions (except sub-titles) are included. The range is broad, so
that a number of nearly-irrelevant titles are included (e.g., Employment
psychology : the Interview , Interviewing in social research , and "Phrasing
questions; the question of bias in interviewing", from Journal of Marketing
).
4. Survey of the Literature on Interrogation Techniques , KUSODA,
1 March 1957, Confidential. Although now somewhat dated because of the
significant work done since its publication, this bibliography remains
the best of those listed. It groups its 114 items in four categories:
Basic Recommended Reading, Recommended Reading, Reading of Limited or
Marginal Value, and Reading of No Value. A brief description of each
item is included. Although some element of subjectivity inevitably tinges
these brief, critical appraisals, they are judicious; and they are also
real time-savers for interrogators too busy to plough through the acres
of print on the specialty.
Contents
XII. Index
A
Abnormalities, spotting of 32
Agents 17
Alice in Wonderland technique 76
All-Seeing Eye technique 67
Anxious, self-centered character 24-25
Arrests 35, 85-86
Assessment, definition of 4
B
Bi-level functioning of interrogator 48
Biographic data 62
Bona fides, definition of 4
C
Character wrecked by success, the 26
Coercive interrogation 82-104
Conclusion of interrogation see
Termination
Confession 38-41, 67, 84
Confinement (see also Deprivation of Sensory Stimuli) 86-87
Confrontation of suspects 47
Control, definition of 4
Conversion 51
Coordination of interrogations 7
Counterintelligence interrogation, definition of 4-5
Cross-examination 58-59
D
Debility 83, 92-93
Debriefing, definition of 5
Defectors 16, 29, 43, 51, 63
Deprivation of sensory stimuli 87-90
Detailed questioning 60-64
Detention of interrogatees 6-8, 49, 86-87
Directives governing interrogation 7
Documents of defectors 36
Double agent 17-18
Drugs (see Narcosis)
Duress (see also Coercive Interrogation)
E
Eliciting, definition of 5
Environment, manipulation of 45-46, 52-53
Escapees 16
Espionage Act 8
Exception, the, as psychological type 27-28
F
Fabricators 18-19
False confessions 94
First children 29
G
Galvanic skin response and the polygraph 80
Going Next Door technique 66
Graphology 81
Greedy-demanding character 23-24
Guilt, feelings of 39, 66, 83
Guilt-ridden character 25-26
H
Heightened suggestibility and hypnosis 95-98
I
Indicators of emotion, physical 54-56
Indirect Assessment Program 30
Informer techniques 67-68
Intelligence interview, definition of 5
Interpreters 74
Interrogatees, emotional needs of
Interrogation, definition of 5
Interrogation, planning of 42-44
Interrogation setting 45-47
Interrogator, desirable characteristics of 10
Interrogator's check list 105-109
Isolation 29
Ivan Is A Dope technique 72
J
Joint Interrogations 4, 43
Joint interrogators, techniques suitable for 47-48, 72-73
Joint suspects 47, 70-72
Judging human nature, fallacies about 12-13
K
Khokhlov, Nikolai 9
L
Language considerations 74
LCFLUTTER 43
Legal considerations affecting KUBARK CI interrogations 6-9
Listening post for interrogations 47
Local laws, importance of 6
M
Magic room technique 77-78
Malingering, detection of 101-102
Matching of interrogation method to source 66
Mindszenty, Cardinal, interrogation of 31
Mutt and Jeff technique 72-73
N
Narcosis 98-100
News from Home technique 68
Nobody Loves You technique 67
Non-coercive interrogation 52-81
O
ODENBY, coordination with 8
Only children 29
Opening the interrogation 53-59
Optimistic character 22-23
Orderly-obstinate character 21-22
Ordinal position 29
Organization of handbook, explanation of 3
Outer and inner office technique 71
P
Pain 90, 93-95
Pauses, significance of 56
PBPRIME citizens, interrogation of 7-8
Penetration agents 11, 18
Personality, categories of 19-28
Personalizing, avoidance of 12
Placebos 77-78
Planning the counterintelligence interrogation 7, 38-44
Police powers, KUBARK's lack of 6-7, 43-44
Policy considerations affecting KUBARK CI interrogations 6-9
Polygraph 79-81
Post-hypnotic suggestion 98
Probing 59-60
Provocateur 11, 17
Purpose of handbook 1-2
R
Rapport, establishment of 10-11, 56
Rationalization 41, 78, 85
Reconnaissance 59-60
Recording of interrogations 46-47
Refugees 16
Regression 40-41, 76-78, 96
Relationship, interrogator-interrogatee 40
Repatriates 15, 42-43
Reports of interrogation 61
Resistance of interrogatees 56-58
Resistance to interrogation 44-45
Respiration rate and the polygraph 80
S
Schizoid character 26-27
Screening 13, 30-33
Separation of interrogatees 47
Silent drugs 97-99
Spinoza and Mortimer Snerd technique 75
Structure of the interrogation 53-65
Swindlers 18-19
Systolic blood pressure and the polygraph 80
T
Techniques of non-coercive interrogation 65-81
Termination of interrogation 50, 63-65
Theory of coercive interrogation 82-84
Threats and fear 90-92
Timing 49-50
Transfer of interrogates to host service 50
Transferred sources 16-17
Trauma 66
Travelers 15
W
Walk-ins 34-36
Witness techniques 68-70
Wolf in Sheep's Clothing technique 75
Contents